WRITINGNEXT
EFFECTIVE STRATEGIES TO IMPROVE
WRITING OF ADOLESCENTS IN MIDDLE
AND HIGH SCHOOLS
By Steve Graham and Dolores Perin
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic
or mechanical, including photocopy, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission
fr
om Car
ne
g
ie Corporation of New York.
A full-text PDF of this document is a
v
ailab
le for fr
ee download
fr
om www
.all4ed.org and www
.car
neg
ie.org/literacy. Additional print copies of this report may be ordered from
the Alliance for Excellent Education at 1201 Connecticut Avenue, NW, Suite 901,Washington, DC 20036,
(202) 828-0828.
Permission for reproducing excerpts from this report should be directed to: Permissions Department, Carnegie
Corporation of Ne
w
York, 437 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10022.
Suggested citation: Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). Writing next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents
in middle and high schools – A report to Carnegie Corporation of New York. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent
Education.
© 2007 Carnegie Corporation of Ne
w York. All rights reserved.
Car
negie Corporation’s
Ad
vancing Literacy
pr
ogram is dedicated to the issues of adolescent literacy and
the research, policy, and practice that focus on the reading and writing competencies of middle and high
school students. Advancing Literacy reports and other publications are designed to encourage local and national
discussion, explore promising ideas, and incubate models of practice, but do not necessarily represent the
recommendations of the Corporation. For more information, visit www.carnegie.org/literacy.
Pub
lished by the Alliance for Excellent Education.
WRITINGNEXT
EFFECTIVE STRATEGIES TO IMPROVE
WRITING OF ADOLESCENTS IN MIDDLE
AND HIGH SCHOOLS
By Steve Graham and Dolores Perin
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
ii
About Carnegie Corporation of New York
Carnegie Corporation of New York was created by Andrew Carnegie in 1911 to promote “the
advancement and diffusion of knowledge and understanding. As a grant-making foundation, the
Corporation seeks to carry out Carnegie’s vision of philanthropy, which he said should aim “to do
real and permanent good in the world. The Corporation’s capital fund, originally donated at a value
of about $135 million, had a market value of $2.2 billion on September 30, 2005.The Corporation
awards grants totaling approximately $80 million a year in the areas of education, international peace
and security, international development, and strengthening U.S. democracy. For more information,
visit www.carnegie.org.
About the Alliance for Excellent Education
Based in Washington, D.C., the Alliance for Excellent Education is a national policy and advocacy
organization that works to ensure that all children graduate from high school prepared for college
and work and to be contributing members of society. It focuses on the needs of the six million
secondary school students (those in the lowest achievement quartile) who are most likely to leave
school without a diploma or to graduate unprepared for a productive future.
The Alliance’s audience includes parents, educators, the federal, state, and local policy communities,
education organizations, business leaders, the media, and a concerned public.To inform the national
debate about education policies and options, the Alliance produces reports and other materials, makes
presentations at meetings and conferences, briefs policymakers and the press, and provides timely
information to a wide audience via its biweekly newsletter and regularly updated website,
www.all4ed.org.
iii
The Authors
Dr. Steve Graham is the Currey Ingram Professor of Special Education and Literacy, a chair he shares
with Karen R. Harris, at Vanderbilt University’s Peabody College of Education. His research interests
include learning disabilities, writing instruction and writing development, and the development of
self-regulation. Graham’s research has focused primarily on identifying the factors that contribute to
the development of writing difficulties; the development and validation of effective procedures for
teaching planning, revising, and the mechanics of writing to struggling writers; and the use of technol-
ogy to enhance writing performance and development. Graham is the editor of
Exceptional Children
and the former editor of Contemporary Educational Psychology. He is coauthor of the Handbook of Writing
Research, Handbook of Learning Disabilities,Writing Better,
and Making the Writing Process Work. He
received an EdD in special education from the University of Kansas. In 2001, Graham was elected a
fellow of the International Academy for Research in Learning Disabilities. He is the recipient of career
research awards from the Council for Exceptional Children and Special Education Research Interest
Group in the American Educational Research Association.
Dr. Dolores Perin is associate professor of psycholo
gy and education at Teachers College, Columbia
University. She directs the Reading Specialist MA Program, which prepares teachers to assess and
teach children and adolescents with reading and writing difficulties. Perin is also a senior research
associate at Teachers College’s Community College Research Center. Her research interests include
the education of academically underprepared students in secondary education, community colleges,
and adult literacy programs, and the preparation of teachers to incorporate literacy strategies in
content-area instruction in middle and high schools. She is principal investigator of studies entitled
“Enhancing Teacher Preparation for Adolescent Literacy through Interdisciplinary Learning
Communities” (Carnegie Corporation of New York) and “Postsecondary Content-Area Reading-
Writing Intervention: Development and Determination of Potential Efficacy” (U.S. Department of
Education Institute of Education Sciences).
Her work also includes studies of community college
de
velopmental education; the integration of academic and career education, funded by the Alfred P.
Sloan F
oundation; and state- and federally-funded projects in school-to-work transition, workplace
literacy
, and adult learning disabilities. Perin received a PhD in psychology from the University of
Sussex in England and is a Ne
w York State-licensed psychologist.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
iv
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Andrés Henríquez, program officer at Carnegie Corporation of New
York, and Cindy Sadler at the Alliance for Excellent Education, who offered helpful suggestions in the
conceptualization and reporting of this research.We wish to thank Joel Levin, Chris Schatschneider,
and Robert Bangert-Drowns for their advice and assistance. Regan Phillips and Beth Chase assisted
in the collection and organization of material for the study, and Paul Morphy served as a second coder
to establish reliability. A special thanks goes to Andrew Wilson and Gina Biancarosa, who took a
120-page document and worked and reworked it to make it suitable for a broad audience.They not
only made the document more readable but contributed important ideas regarding how this work
should be framed and presented. It was a pleasure to work with them.They are both consummate
professionals.The order of authorship is alphabetical.
Steve Graham
Dolor
es P
erin
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
v
CONTENTS
Foreword by Vartan Gregorian........................................................................................................1
Executive Summary.........................................................................................................................3
Introduction.....................................................................................................................................7
Recommendations: 11 Key Elements of Effective Adolescent Writing Instruction
as Identified by Meta-analysis
.......................................................................................................11
Implementing the Elements..........................................................................................................22
Accumulation of Research on Adolescent Writing Instruction..................................................25
A Challenge...................................................................................................................................28
References......................................................................................................................................29
Appendix A: Meta-analysis Methodology.....................................................................................35
Appendix B:
Quasi-experimental and Experimental Studies
Suppor
ting the 11 Key Elements
..................................................................................................
43
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
vi
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
1
FOREWORD
Around the world, from the cave paintings in Lascaux, France, which may be 25,000 years old, to the
images left behind by the lost Pueblo cultures of the American Southwest, to the ancient aboriginal
art of Australia, the most common pictograph found in rock paintings is the human hand. Coupled
with pictures of animals, with human forms, with a starry night sky or other images that today we can
only identify as abstract, we look at these men’s and women’s hands, along with smaller prints that
perhaps belong to childr
en, and cannot help but be deeply moved by the urge of our ancestors to
lea
ve some permanent imprint of themselves behind.
Clearly, the instinct for human beings to express their feelings, their thoughts, and their experiences
in some lasting form has been with us for a very long time.This urge eventually manifested itself in
the creation of the first alphabet, which many attribute to the Phoenicians.When people also began
to recognize the concept of time, their desire to express themselves became intertwined with the
sense of wanting to leave behind a legacy, a message about who they were, what they had done and
seen, and even what they believed in.Whether inscribed on rock, carved in cuneiform, painted in
hieroglyphics, or written with the aid of the alphabet, the instinct to write down everything from
mundane commercial transactions to routine daily occurrences to the most transcendent ideas—and
then to ha
v
e other
s read them, as well as to read what others have written—is not simply a way of
transfer
r
ing infor
mation fr
om one per
son to another, one generation to the next. It is a process of
lear
ning and hence
,
of education.
Ariel and Will Durant were right when they said,“Education is the transmission of civilization.
Putting our current challenges into historical context, it is obvious that if today’s youngsters cannot
read with understanding, think about and analyze what they’ve read, and then write clearly and
effectively about what they’ve learned and what they think, then they may never be able to do justice
to their talents and their potential. (In that regard, the etymology of the word
education, which is to
draw out and draw forth—from oneself, for example—is certainly evocative.) Indeed, young people
who do not have the ability to transform thoughts, experiences, and ideas into written words are in
danger of losing touch with the joy of inquiry, the sense of intellectual curiosity, and the inestimable
satisfaction of acquiring wisdom that are the touchstones of humanity.What that means for all of us is
that the essential educative transmissions that have been passed along century after century, generation
after generation,
ar
e in danger of f
ading a
w
a
y
, or even falling silent.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
2
In a recent report, the National Commission on Writing also addresses this concern.They say, “If
students are to make knowledge their own, they must struggle with the details, wrestle with the facts,
and rework raw information and dimly understood concepts into language they can communicate to
someone else. In short, if students are to learn, they must write.
It is in this connection that I am pleased to introduce
Writing Next. As the report warns, American
students today are not meeting even basic writing standards, and their teachers are often at a loss for
how to help them. In an age overwhelmed by information (we are told, for example, that all available
information doubles every two to three years), we should view this as a crisis, because the ability to
r
ead, comprehend, and write—in other words, to organize information into
knowledge—can be vie
wed
as tantamount to a survival skill.Why? Because in the decades ahead, Americans face yet another
challenge: how to keep our democracy and our society from being divided not only between rich
and poor, but also between those who ha
ve access to information and knowledge, and thus, to
power—the power of enlightenment,
the po
wer of self-improvement and self-assertion, the power to
achieve upw
ard mobility
, and the power over their own lives and their families’ ability to thrive and
succeed—and those who do not.
Such an uncrossable divide will have devastating consequences for the future of America.Those who
enrich themselves by learning to read with understanding and write with skill and clarity do so not
only for themselves and their families, but for our nation as well.They learn in order to preserve and
enhance the record of humanity, to be productive members of a larger community, to be good citizens
and good ancestors to those who will follow after them. In an age of globalization, when economies
sink or swim on their ability to mine and manage knowledge, as do both individual and national
security, we cannot afford to let this generation of ours or indeed, any other, fall behind the learning
curve. Let me bring us back to where we began: For all of us, the handprint must remain firmly and
clearly on the wall.
Vartan Gregorian
President, Carnegie Corporation of New York
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
3
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY
A Writing Proficiency Crisis
Writing well is not just an option for young people—it is a necessity. Along with reading comprehen-
sion, writing skill is a predictor of academic success and a basic requirement for participation in civic
life and in the global economy.Yet every year in the United States large numbers of adolescents gradu-
ate from high school unable to write at the basic levels required by colleges or employers. In addition,
every school day 7,000 young people drop out of high school (Alliance for Excellent Education,
2006), many of them because they lack the basic literacy skills to meet the growing demands of the
high school curriculum (Kamil, 2003; Snow & Biancarosa, 2003). Because the definition of
literacy
includes both reading and writing skills, poor writing proficiency should be recognized as an intrinsic
part of this national literacy crisis.
This report offers a number of specific teaching techniques that research suggests will help 4th- to
12th-grade students in our nation’s schools.The report focuses on all students, not just those who
display writing difficulties, although this latter group is deservedly the focus of much attention.The
premise of this report is that all students need to become proficient and flexible writers. In this report,
the term
low-achieving writers is used to refer to students whose writing skills are not adequate to meet
classroom demands. Some of these low-achieving writers have been identified as having learning
disabilities;
others are the “silent majority” who lack writing proficiency but do not receive additional
help
. As will be seen in this report, some studies investigate the effects of writing instruction on groups
of students acr
oss the full range of ability, from more effective to less effective writers, while others
focus specifically on indi
viduals with low writing proficiency.
Recent r
epor
ts b
y the National Commission on
Writing (2003, 2004, 2005) have helped to bring
the impor
tance of wr
iting pr
oficiency forw
ard into the public consciousness.These reports provide
a jumping-off point for thinking about ho
w to impr
o
v
e writing instruction for all young people,
with a special focus on str
uggling r
eader
s.
Reading Next (Biancar
osa & Sno
w
,
2004), commissioned by
Carneg
ie Cor
poration of New York, used up-to-date research to highlight a number of key elements
seen as essential to impr
o
ving r
eading instruction for adolescents (defined as grades 4–12).
Wr
iting
Next
sets out to pr
o
vide guidance for impr
oving writing instruction for adolescents, a topic that has
previously not r
eceived enough attention from researchers or educators.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
4
While Reading Next presented general methods and interventions that several of America’s most
respected adolescent literacy experts found to be useful for improving reading instruction,
Writing
Next
highlights specific teaching techniques that work in the classroom. It does so by summarizing
the results of a large-scale statistical review of research into the effects of specific types of writing
instruction on adolescents’ writing proficiency. Although several important reviews of research on
writing instruction exist (e.g., Langer & Applebee, 1987; Levy & Ransdell, 1996; MacArthur, Graham,
& Fitzgerald, 2006; Smagorinsky, 2006), the special strength of this report is its use of a powerful
statistical method known as meta-analysis.This technique allows researchers to determine the
consistency and strength of the effects of instructional practices on student writing quality and to
highlight those practices that hold the most promise.
The Recommendations
Eleven Elements of Effective Adolescent Writing Instruction
This report identifies 11 elements of current writing instruction found to be effective for helping
adolescent students learn to write well and to use writing as a tool for learning. It is important to note
that all of the elements are supported by rigorous research, but that even when used together, they do
not constitute a full writing curriculum.
1. Writing Strategies, which involves teaching students strategies for planning, revising, and
editing their compositions
2. Summarization, which involves explicitly and systematically teaching students how to
summarize texts
3. Collaborative Writing, which uses instructional arrangements in which adolescents work
together to plan, draft, revise, and edit their compositions
4. Specific Product Goals, which assigns students specific, reachable goals for the writing they
ar
e to complete
5.
W
or
d Pr
ocessing,
which uses computer
s and w
or
d pr
ocessors as instructional supports for
writing assignments
6. Sentence Combining, which involves teaching students to construct more complex,
sophisticated sentences
7. Prewriting, which engages students in activities designed to help them generate or organize
ideas for their composition
8. Inquir
y
Acti
vities
,
which engages students in analyzing immediate
,
concr
ete data to help
them develop ideas and content for a particular writing task
9. Process Writing Approach, which interweaves a number of writing instructional activities in
a w
orkshop en
vir
onment that str
esses extended wr
iting oppor
tunities, writing for authentic
audiences, personalized instruction, and cycles of writing
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
5
10. Study of Models, which provides students with opportunities to read, analyze, and emulate
models of good writing
11. Writing for Content Learning, which uses writing as a tool for learning content material
The
Writing Next elements do not constitute a full writing curriculum, any more than the Reading
Next
elements did for reading. However, all of the Writing Next instructional elements have shown
clear results for improving students’ writing.They can be combined in flexible ways to strengthen
adolescents’ literacy development.The authors hope that besides providing research-supported
information about effective writing instruction for classroom teachers, this report will stimulate
discussion and action at policy and research levels, leading to solid improvements in writing
instruction in grades 4 to 12 nationwide.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
6
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
7
INTRODUCTION
Although the nation has made progress recently in improving
the literacy achievement of its elementary school students,
adolescent literacy levels have remained stagnant (Lemke et al.,
2004; National Center for Education Statistics, 1999, 2006;
Olson, 2006). As a result, attention has begun to turn to the need
to impr
ove the literacy of adolescent students. One example of
this ne
w focus is the recently created Striving Readers Initiative,
a federal pr
ogram to help school districts meet the challenge of
improving adolescents’ literacy skills, for which the U.S. Congress
appropriated just over $29 million for the 2006–07 school year.
Several reports have drawn attention to the adolescent literacy
crisis (e.g., Kamil, 2003; American Diploma Project, 2004;
Carnevale, 2001; National Commission on Writing, 2004).
Among them,
Reading Next outlined elements of literacy
instruction with a strong track record of positive results among
adolescents (Biancar
osa & Sno
w
, 2004).While these reports
and other
s ha
v
e br
ought m
uch-needed attention to adolescents’
literacy needs,
the
y w
er
e concer
ned more with reading than
with wr
iting skills.
Lo
w-Achie
ving
Wr
iters: Scope of the Problem
Wr
iting is sometimes seen as the
“flip side”
of r
eading. It is
often assumed that adolescents who ar
e pr
oficient r
eader
s must
be pr
oficient wr
iter
s,
too. If this were the case, then helping
students lear
n to r
ead better w
ould naturally lead to the same
students wr
iting w
ell.
However, although reading and writing
are complementar
y skills whose de
velopment runs a roughly
parallel cour
se
,
they do not necessarily go hand in hand. Many
adolescents ar
e ab
le to handle a
verage reading demands but have severe difficulties with writing.
Moreov
er, the nature of the relationship between reading and writing skills changes over time
CAUSE FOR ALARM
Seventy percent of
students in grades 4–12
are low-achieving writers
(Persky et al., 2003).
Every school day, more
than 7,000 students drop out
of high school (Pinkus,
2006).
Only
70% of high school
students graduate on time
with a regular diploma,
and fewer than 60% of
African-American and
Latino students do so
(Greene & Winters, 2005).
Students who enter ninth
grade in
the lowest 25%
of their class are 20 times
more likely to drop out than
are the highest-performing
students (Carnevale, 2001).
Nearly one third
of high
school graduates ar
e
not ready for college-level
English composition
courses (ACT
, 2005).
Over
half of adults scoring
at the lowest literacy
levels are dropouts, and
almost a quarter of these
persons are high school
graduates
(National Center
for Education Statistics,
2005).
Continued on Page 8
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
8
(Fitzgerald & Shanahan, 2000). Researchers know that reading
and writing often draw from the same pool of background
knowledge—for example, a general understanding of the
attributes of texts. At the same time, however, writing differs from
reading.While readers form a mental representation of thoughts
written by someone else, writers formulate their own thoughts,
organize them, and create a written record of them using the
conventions of spelling and grammar.
Therefore, although writing and reading are both vital aspects of
literacy, they each require their own dedicated instruction.What
improves reading does not always improve writing.This report
responds to the strong need for information about how to
improve classroom writing instruction to address the serious problem of adolescent writing difficulty.
The National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) writing
exam was last given in 2002 (Persky, Daane, & Jin, 2003); it
measured the writing skills of 4th, 8th, and 12th graders and
translated their scores into three levels of proficiency: Basic,
Proficient, or Advanced. A disturbing finding was that only 22%
to 26% of students scored at the Proficient level across the three
grades, and very few were found to write at the Advanced level (Persky et al., 2003,Table 2.1). Even
worse, alarmingly high proportions of students were found to be at or below the Basic level. Not
only did 15% of 4th and 8th graders and 26% of 12th graders test below the Basic level, but 58%,
54%, and 51% of students, respectively, at these grade levels tested at the Basic level. In sum, 72% of
4th-grade students, 69% of 8th-grade students, and 77% of 12th-grade students did not meet NAEP
writing proficiency goals.
These results clearly demonstrate that very large numbers of adolescents need interventions to help
them become better writers. Some, especially those who score at or below the Basic level on the
NAEP, require more help than others.
Consequences
A wide range of jobs require employees to produce written documentation, visual/text presentations,
memoranda, technical reports, and electronic messages.The explosion of electronic and wireless
communication in everyday life brings writing skills into play as never before. Recent reports by
the National Commission on Writing (2004, 2005) reveal that the majority of both public and
private employers say that writing proficiency has now become critical in the workplace and that it
directly affects hiring and promotion decisions.The demand for writing proficiency is not limited to
CAUSE FOR ALARM
College instructors estimate
that 50% of high school
graduates are not prepared
for college-level writing
(Achieve, Inc., 2005).
U.S. graduates’
literacy
skills are lower than
those of graduates in most
industrialized nations,
comparable only to the
skills of graduates in Chile,
Poland, Portugal, and
Slovenia (OECD, 2000).
The knowledge and skills
required for higher education
and for employment are now
considered equivalent (ACT,
2006; American Diploma
Project, 2004).
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
9
professional jobs but extends to clerical and support positions
in government, construction, manufacturing, service industries,
and elsewhere. In fact, about 30% of government and private
sector employees require on-the-job training in basic writing
skills. Private companies spend an estimated $3.1 billion annually
on remediation, and state governments spend an estimated $221
million annually (National Commission on Writing, 2005).
Young people who have difficulty writing are not fully equipped
to meet the demands of college, either. A recent study by ACT
(2005) revealed that about a third of high school students
intending to enter higher education do not meet readiness
benchmarks for college-level English composition courses
(among certain ethnic groups, 50% or more of adolescents do
not meet ACT benchmarks), making it unlikely that they will
be able to learn effectively in the college setting.
Many students begin postsecondary education at a community
college. However, at least a quarter of new community college
students enroll in remedial writing courses (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2003). Compounding the problem, remedial
enrollments appear to underestimate the number of students
who actually need help with writing (Perin, 2006). Community
colleges have always attempted to meet the needs of students with
reading and writing difficulties, and many would argue that doing so is a core part of their mission.
Many 2-year institutions find it difficult, however; they are not equipped to teach writing effectively
to such large numbers of students, and the presence of students with poor academic skills in their
classr
ooms can under
mine the quality of the r
egular academic cur
r
iculum (Gr
ubb et al.,
1999;
Perin &
Char
r
on,
2006).
Wh
y
Wr
iting Is Impor
tant
Most contexts of life (school,
the w
orkplace
,
and the comm
unity) call for some le
v
el of writing skill,
and each context mak
es o
v
erlapping,
b
ut not identical,
demands.
Proficient writers can adapt their
wr
iting flexib
ly to the context in which it tak
es place
.
In the school setting, writing plays two distinct but complementary roles. First, it is a skill that draws
on the use of strategies (such as planning, evaluating, and revising text) to accomplish a variety of
goals, such as writing a report or expressing an opinion with the support of evidence. Second, writing
is a means of extending and deepening students’ knowledge; it acts as a tool for learning subject matter
WRITING IN THE
WORKPLACE
Thirty-five percent of high
school graduates in college
and 38% of high school
graduates in the workforce
feel their writing does not
meet expectations for quality
(Achieve, Inc., 2005).
About half of private
employers and more than
60% of state government
employers say
writing
skills impact promotion
decisions
(National
Commission on Writing,
2004, 2005).
Poorly written applications
are likely to doom
candidates’ chances for
employment
(National
Commission on Writing,
2005, p. 4).
Writing remediation costs
American businesses as
much as $3.1 billion annually
(National Commission on
Writing, 2004).
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
10
(Keys, 2000; Shanahan, 2004; Sperling & Freedman, 2001). Because these roles are closely linked,
Reading Next recommended that language arts teachers use content-area texts to teach reading and
writing skills and that content-area teachers provide instruction and practice in discipline-specific
reading and writing.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
11
RECOMMENDATIONS: 11 KEY ELEMENTS OF
EFFECTIVE ADOLESCENT WRITING INSTRUCTION
AS IDENTIFIED BY META-ANALYSIS
This report provides long-needed guidance for teachers and policymakers by identifying specific
instructional practices that improve the quality of adolescent students’ writing.The special contribution
of this report is that it draws from empirical evidence.
The authors set out to collect, categorize, and analyze experimental and quasi-experimental research
on adolescent writing instruction in order to determine which elements of existing instructional
methods are reported to be effective by research.The method used, meta-analysis, provides a measure
of effectiveness using the effect size statistic. On the basis of the effect sizes found,
Writing Next
presents 11 elements of effective adolescent writing instruction. (A detailed description of the
methodolo
gy used is found in Appendix A.)
No single approach to writing instruction will meet the needs of all students. Also, some extant
techniques may be effective but have not yet been studied rigorously.There is a tremendous need
for more research on and dissemination of adolescent writing interventions that work, so that adminis-
trators and teachers can select the strategies that are most appropriate, whether for whole classrooms,
small groups, or individual students.
Though each instr
uctional element is tr
eated as a distinct entity
, the different elements are often
r
elated,
and the addition of one element can stim
ulate the inclusion of another. In an ideal world,
teacher
s w
ould be ab
le to incorporate all of the 11 key elements in their everyday writing curricula,
b
ut the list ma
y also be used to constr
uct a unique blend of elements suited to specific student needs.
The elements should not be seen as isolated b
ut rather as interlink
ed.
For instance, it is difficult to
implement the pr
ocess wr
iting approach (element 9) without having peers work together (element 3)
or use pr
e
writing supports (element 7). A mixture of these elements is likely to generate the
biggest r
etur
n. It remains to be seen what that optimal mix is, and it may be different for different
subpopulations of students.
Ho
wever, it is important to stress that these 11 elements are not meant
to constitute a cur
r
iculum.
1. Writing Strategies
2. Summarization
3.
Collaborative Writing
4. Specific Product Goals
5. Word Processing
6. Sentence-Combining
7. Prewriting
8. Inquiry Activities
9.
Process Writing Approach
10. Study of Models
11. Writing for Content Learning
Effective Elements to Impr
ove Writing Achievement in Grades 4 to 12
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
12
The instructional elements are ordered according to their average
effect.Therefore, elements with larger effect sizes are presented
before those with smaller effect sizes. However, many of the
effect sizes differ only minimally, so readers should be cautious
in interpreting the differences in effect strength. Appendix B lists
references for the studies used in determining the elements, in the
same order as the elements.
The report’s findings are based strictly on experimental and
quasi-experimental research, as this is the only type of research
that allows for rigorous comparison of effects across studies.
While a range of methodologies have been used to study
writing—from research into the history of writing instruction
to surveys of student attitudes about writing to studies that aim
to describe the actions of particularly successful teachers—there
have been few efforts to compare the effectiveness of specific
teaching strategies. Meta-analysis fills this gap.
It is also important to note that the findings in this report are
cumulative, in that they build on earlier meta-analyses of writing
instruction (Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Bangert-Drowns, Hurley, &
Wilkinson, 2004; Goldberg, Russell, & Cook, 2003; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003; Hillocks,
1986).This report includes all of the studies of adolescents reviewed in the prior meta-analyses.
Further, the report adapts some of the earlier authors’ categorizations of instruction, such as some
of those used by Hillocks (1986). In addition, these earlier meta-analyses have been considerably
extended by (a) updating the earlier findings; (b) reorganizing earlier instructional categories to
incorporate newer findings; and (c) examining the impact of instruction more recently studied.
Benefits of Meta-analytic
Appr
oach
By their v
er
y natur
e
,
meta-analyses ar
e concer
ned with quantitative data; as noted above, this report
look
ed at exper
imental and quasi-exper
imental r
esear
ch on wr
iting instr
uction. Its conclusions
should in no w
a
y detract fr
om the impor
tant contr
ib
utions that other types of r
esearch make to
an under
standing of ho
w to teach wr
iting.
F
or instance
,
the report’s conclusions do not reflect the
findings fr
om a n
umber of excellent obser
v
ational studies that examine the wr
iting practices of
effective teacher
s of wr
iting (e
.g.,
Pressley,Yokoi, Rankin,Wharton-McDonald, & Mistretta, 1997),
studies that measur
e the cor
r
elations betw
een wr
iting perfor
mance and particular teaching procedures
(e.g., Applebee
,
Langer
,
Nystrand, & Gamoran, 2003), or single-subject design studies (e.g., De La Paz,
1999). Like
wise
,
man
y perspectives, including cognitive (Hayes, 2000), sociocultural (Prior, 2006), and
discourse (Chafe &
T
annen,
1987),
infor
m the study of writing (Sperling & Freedman, 2001).
THE OPTIMAL MIX
In the medical profession,
treatment is tailored to
individual patient needs;
at times, more than one
intervention is needed to
effectively treat a patient.
Similarly, educators need
to test mixes of intervention
elements to find the ones
that work best for students
with different needs.
Researchers do not know
what combination or
how much of each of the
r
ecommended activities is
needed to maximize writing
instruction for adolescents
in general or low-achieving
writers in particular. Nor
do they yet know what
combination of elements
works for which types
of writers.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
13
Although these viewpoints
were not equally represented
in the research studies included
in this analysis, each is critical
to understanding writing
development. Finally, the
recently published third edition
of
Research on Composition
(Smagorinsky, 2006) provides
a broad overview of the field—
covering topics such as
rhetoric, second language
writing, multimodal composi-
tion, and home and workplace
writing—and a survey of
research and theory over the
past 20 years (see also
Handbook
of Writing Research
; MacArthur,
Graham, & Fitzgerald, 2006).
With such a wide range of
wr
iting instr
uction practices
and per
specti
ves, this review
of the literatur
e aims not to
descr
ibe the full context of the
high-functioning classr
oom b
ut
to pr
o
vide specific practices
that ha
v
e demonstrated effecti
v
eness across a number of
contexts—a pur
pose to which meta-analysis is ideally suited.
F
or an
y of the practices r
eviewed, contexts can vary widely.
F
or instance
,
they may include any grade between 4th and 12th;
the
y ma
y or ma
y not be inclusive classrooms serving students
with lear
ning disabilities or wr
iting in their second language;
and
the
y ma
y in
volve teachers with very different beliefs about what
good wr
iting instr
uction entails. However, meta-analysis allows
consideration of both the str
ength and consistency of a practice’
s effects.
A TECHNICAL NOTE ON META-ANALYSIS
What is a Meta-analysis?
Meta-analysis is a particularly powerful way of synthesizing large
bodies of research, as it relies on quantitative studies and permits
the calculation of effect sizes. The strength of meta-analysis as
an approach is that it allows consideration of both the strength
and consistency of a practice’s effects.
What is an Effect Size?
Effect sizes report the average difference between a type of
instruction and a comparison condition. They indicate the
strength of the effect. The following guidelines make these
numbers mor
e meaningful.
0.20 =
small or mild effect
0.50 =
medium or moderate ef
fect
0.80 =
large or strong ef
fect
Positive effect sizes mean the instruction had a positive effect on
student writing.
Negative effect sizes mean the instruction had a negative effect
on student writing.
Although these guidelines are commonly accepted, it is important
to interpr
et effect sizes within the context of a given field. For
instance, the National Reading Panel report (National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development, 2000) found an effect size
of 0.53 for phonemic awareness instruction, while effect sizes for
fluency instruction ranged from 0.35 to 0.50. More research is
needed to establish the range of effect sizes for writing strategies
identified in the current meta-analysis.
Also, it is important to note that the large number of factors that
affect adolescent literacy outcomes and the difficulty in improving
writing ability render
any significant effect meaningful.
Appendix A sets out the
methodology used in the
meta-analysis. Appendix B
lists all of the categories for
which four or more studies
were analyzed and provides
descriptive information about
each study.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
14
T
he Outcome of Writing Instruction
The authors followed in the footsteps of previous researchers by using writing quality as the outcome
studied.Writing quality is defined here in terms of coherently organized essays containing well-
developed and pertinent ideas, supporting examples, and appropriate detail (Needels & Knapp, 1994).
Writing quality was included as the primary outcome, or one of several primary outcomes, in all
previous meta-analyses on procedures for teaching writing (Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Goldberg et al.,
2003; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003; Hillocks, 1986).Writing quality served as the sole
outcome measure because the authors were interested in identifying treatments that had a broad
impact on writing performance.The only exceptions involved studies examining the teaching of
summarization, in which completeness and accuracy of summaries were assessed, and writing-to-learn
studies, in which content learning was the outcome measure.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
15
The 11 Key Elements of Adolescent Writing Instruction
Writing Strategies (Effect Size = 0.82)
Teaching adolescents strategies for planning, revising, and editing their compositions has shown a
dramatic effect on the quality of students’ writing. Strategy instruction involves explicitly and
systematically teaching steps necessary for planning, revising, and/or editing text (Graham, 2006).
The ultimate goal is to teach students to use these strategies independently.
Strategy instruction may involve teaching more generic processes, such as brainstorming (e.g.,Troia &
Graham, 2002) or collaboration for peer revising (MacArthur, Schwartz, & Graham, 1991). In other
instances, it involves teaching strategies for accomplishing specific types of writing tasks, such as
writing a story (Fitzgerald &
Markham, 1987) or a
persuasive essay (Yeh, 1998).
Whether generic or highly
focused, explicitly teaching
adolescents strategies for
planning, revising, and/or
editing has a strong impact on
the quality of their writing.
Writing strategy instruction
has been found especially
effective for adolescents who
have difficulty writing, but it
is also a powerful technique
for adolescents in general. For
example, 11 studies with low-
achieving writers and 9 studies
with students representing
normal variation within the
classroom were reviewed.
The average weighted effect
size for the studies with
low-achieving writers (1.02)
was larger than the average
weighted effect size for
students across the full
range of ability in regular
classrooms (0.70).
WRITING STRATEGIES: AN EXAMPLE
Self-Regulated Strategy Development (SRSD) is an approach
for helping students learn specific strategies for planning,
drafting, and revising text. SRSD instruction is also characterized
by explicit teaching, individualized instruction, and criterion-
based versus time-based learning. Children are treated as active
collaborators in the learning process. Instruction takes place in
six stages:
Develop Background Knowledge: Students are taught any back-
ground knowledge needed to use the strategy successfully.
Describe It: The strategy as well as its purpose and benefits is
described and discussed.
Model It: The teacher models how to use the strategy.
Memorize It: The student memorizes the steps of the strategy
and any accompanying mnemonic.
Suppor
t It
: The teacher suppor
ts or scaffolds student mastery of
the strategy.
Independent Use: Students use the strategy with few or no
supports.
Students are also taught a number of self-regulation skills
(including goal setting, self-monitoring, self-instruction, and
self-reinforcement) designed to help them manage writing
strategies, the writing process, and their behavior. Mnemonics
are introduced to help students remember strategies to increase
writing per
for
mance. T
wo such strategies ar
e PLAN and WRITE:
PLAN (
Pay attention to the prompt, List the main idea, Add
suppor
ting ideas, Number your ideas)
WRITE (
Work from your plan to develop your thesis statement,
Remember your goals, Include transition words for each
paragraph, Try to use different kinds of sentences, and Exciting,
interesting, $10,000 words).
Sour
ces: De La Paz & Graham, 2002; Harris & Graham, 1996
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
16
Self-Regulated Strategy Development (SRSD) is a particularly effective approach for teaching
writing strategies.The average weighted effect size for SRSD studies (1.14) was larger than for
non-SRSD studies (0.62). SRSD is characterized by explicit instruction of writing strategies and
self-regulation procedures (e.g., self-assessment and goal setting), as well as individualized instruction
and criterion-based learning (see box above).
Strategy instruction is well supported by research. Its effects appear to be more dramatic for lower-
achieving writers than for those across the full range of ability. Although SRSD had stronger effects
than most other strategy approaches, the meta-analysis indicates moderate to strong effects of writing
strategy instruction in general.
Summarization (Effect Size = 0.82)
Writing instruction often involves explicitly and systematically teaching students how to summarize
texts.The summarization approaches studied ranged from explicitly teaching summarization strategies
to enhancing summarization by progressively “fading” models of a good summary. In fact, students can
learn to write better summaries from either a rule-governed or a more intuitive approach. Overall,
teaching adolescents to summarize text had a consistent, strong, positive effect on their ability to write
good summaries.
Collaborative Writing (Effect Size = 0.75)
Collaborative wr
iting involves developing instructional arrangements whereby adolescents work
to
gether to plan,
draft, revise, and edit their compositions. It shows a strong impact on improving the
quality of students’
wr
iting.
Studies of this approach
compared its effectiveness
with that of having students
compose independently
.
The
effect sizes for all studies w
er
e
positi
v
e and large
.
Collecti
v
ely
,
these in
v
estigations sho
w that
collaborati
v
e ar
rangements in
which students help each other
with one or mor
e aspects of
their writing ha
v
e a str
ong
positive impact on quality
.
It w
as not possib
le to draw separate conclusions for low-achieving writers,
as only two studies (Daile
y
,
1991;
Macar
thur et al., 1991) involved these students specifically. However,
in both studies the effect size exceeded 1.00.
COLLABORATIVE WRITING: ONE APPROACH
Collaborative writing involves peers writing as a team. In one
approach, a higher achieving student is assigned to be the
Helper (tutor) and a lower achieving student is assigned to be
the Writer (tutee). The students are instructed to work as partners
on a writing task. The Helper student assists the W
riter student
with meaning, organization, spelling, punctuation, generating
ideas, creating a draft, rereading essays, editing essays, choosing
the best copy
, and evaluating the final pr
oduct. Thr
oughout the
inter
vention, the teacher’
s role is to monitor, prompt, and praise
the students, and addr
ess their concer
ns.
Source: Yarrow & Topping, 2001
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
17
S
pecific Product Goals (Effect Size = 0.70)
Setting product goals involves assigning students specific, reachable goals for the writing they are
to complete. It includes identifying the purpose of the assignment (e.g., to persuade) as well as
characteristics of the final product.
Specific goals in the studies
reviewed included (a) adding
more ideas to a paper when
revising, or establishing a goal
to write a specific kind of
paper and (b) assigning goals
for specific structural elements
in a composition. Compared
with instances in which
students were simply given
a general overall goal, these
relatively simple procedures resulted in a positive effect size, and the average effect was strong. It was
possible to obtain effect sizes specifically for low-achieving writers in three of the five product goal
studies (which involved disaggregating results reported in Ferretti, MacArthur, & Dowdy, 2000).
The average effect for these students was similarly strong, providing some tentative evidence that,
inter
preted cautiously (because of the small sample), indicates that setting pr
oduct goals is effective
with adolescents who are weaker wr
iters. Overall, assigning students goals for their written product
had a str
ong impact on wr
iting quality.
Word Processing (Effect Size = 0.55)
The use of word-processing equipment can be particularly helpful for low-achieving writers. In this
type of instruction, students might work collaboratively on writing assignments using personal laptop
computers, or they might learn to word-process a composition under teacher guidance.Typing text on
the computer with word-processing software produces a neat and legible script. It allows the writer to
add, delete, and move text easily.Word-processing software, especially in more recent studies, includes
spell checkers as well.
Compared with composing by hand, the effect of word-processing instruction in most of the studies
reviewed was positive, suggesting that word processing has a consistently positive impact on writing
quality.The average effect on writing quality was moderate for students in general (effect size = 0.51),
but for low-achieving writers it was larger (effect size = 0.70).Thus, word processing appears to be
an effective instructional support for students in grades 4 to 12 and may be especially effective in
enhancing the quality of text produced by low-achieving writers.
SETTING SPECIFIC PRODUCT GOALS:
ONE APPROACH
Setting specific product goals provides students with objectives
to focus on particular aspects of their writing. For example,
students may be instructed to take a position and write a
persuasive letter designed to lead an audience to agree with
them. In addition to this general goal, teachers provide explicit
subgoals on argumentative discourse, including a statement
of belief, two or three reasons for that belief, examples or
supporting information for each reason, two or three reasons
why others might disagree, and why those reasons are incorrect.
Source: Ferretti, MacArthur, & Dowdy, 2000
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
18
S
entence Combining (Effect Size = 0.50)
Sentence combining involves
teaching students to construct
more complex and sophisticat-
ed sentences through exercises
in which two or more basic
sentences are combined into
a single sentence.Teaching
adolescents how to write
increasingly complex sentences
in this way enhances the
quality of their writing. Studies
establishing the effectiveness of
sentence combining primarily
compared it with more traditional grammar instruction.The effect sizes for all studies were
consistently positive and moderate in strength.
Only one study (Saddler & Graham, 2005) examined the effects of sentence combining on low-
achieving writers.When the effects of sentence combining were disaggregated for different types
of writers in this study (low-achieving and average writers), the effect size for the weaker writers
was 0.46. Overall,
the current analysis of sentence combining indicates that this focus of instruction
has a moderate impact on improving the quality of the writing of adolescents in general.
Pr
e-wr
iting (Effect Size = 0.32)
Pre-writing engages students in activities designed to help them generate or organize ideas for their
composition. Engaging adolescents in such activities before they write a first draft improves the quality
of their writing. Pre-writing activities include gathering possible information for a paper through
reading or developing a visual representation of their ideas before sitting down to write. For example,
some common pre-writing activities include encouraging group and individual planning before
writing, organizing pre-writing ideas, prompting students to plan after providing a brief demonstration
of how to do so, or assigning reading material pertinent to a topic and then encouraging students to
plan their work in advance. It was not possible to draw separate conclusions for low-achieving writers,
as all of the pre-writing studies involved students across the full range of ability in regular classrooms.
Collectively, these investigations show that pre-writing activities have a positive and small to moderate
impact on the quality of students’ writing.
SENTENCE-COMBINING: ONE APPROACH
Sentence-combining is an alternative approach to more
traditional grammar instruction. Sentence-combining instruction
involves teaching students to construct more complex and
sophisticated sentences through exercises in which two or
more basic sentences are combined into a single sentence.
In one approach, students at higher and lower writing levels
are paired to receive six lessons that teach (a) combining
smaller related sentences into a compound sentence using the
connectors
and, but, and because; (b) embedding an adjective
or adverb from one sentence into another; (c) creating complex
sentences by embedding an adverbial and adjectival clause from
one sentence into another; and (d) making multiple embeddings
involving adjectives, adverbs, adverbial clauses, and adjectival
clauses. The instructor provides support and modeling and the
student pairs work collaboratively to apply the skills taught.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
19
I
nquiry Activities (Effect Size = 0.32)
Inquiry means engaging
students in activities that
help them develop ideas and
content for a particular writing
task by analyzing immediate,
concrete data (comparing and
contrasting cases or collecting
and evaluating evidence).
Involving adolescents in
writing activities designed to
sharpen their inquiry skills
improves the quality of their
writing. Effective inquiry
activities in writing are
characterized by a clearly specified goal (e.g., describe the actions of people), analysis of concrete
and immediate data (observe one or more peers during specific activities), use of specific strategies
to conduct the analysis (retrospectively ask the person being observed the reason for a particular
action), and applying what was learned (assign the writing of a story incorporating insights from
the inquiry pr
ocess).
It was found that this type of instr
uction was last studied in 1986.
The comparison conditions in
the inquir
y studies w
ere relatively similar, primarily involving writing activities facilitated by teachers.
It w
as not possib
le to dra
w any specific conclusions for low-achieving writers, as all of the studies
involved the full range of students in a typical classroom. Despite the lack of new research, the
evidence suggests that engaging students in inquiry activities in which they analyze data before
writing is an effective instructional practice.
Process Writing Approach (Effect Size = 0.32)
The process writing approach involves a number of interwoven activities, including creating
extended opportunities for writing; emphasizing writing for real audiences; encouraging cycles
of planning, translating, and reviewing; stressing personal responsibility and ownership of writing
projects; facilitating high levels of student interactions; developing supportive writing environments;
encouraging self-reflection and evaluation; and offering personalized individual assistance, brief
instructional lessons to meet students’ individual needs, and, in some instances, more extended and
systematic instruction.The overall effect of the process writing approach was small to moderate, but
significant. Only three studies specifically examined the impact of the process writing approach with
low-achieving writers, making it difficult to draw any conclusions about its efficacy for these students.
INQUIRY ACTIVITIES: AN EXAMPLE
Students examine and infer the qualities of a number of objects
in order to describe them in writing. The students touch objects
while wearing blindfolds, examine seashells, listen to sounds,
do physical exercise, become aware of bodily sensations,
examine pictures, pantomime brief scenarios, act out dialogues,
and examine model compositions. Students’ responses to these
objects are elicited. Students list more and more precise details,
and respond to each other’s descriptions in small groups or whole
classes under teacher guidance in order to become increasingly
aware of the writing task and possible audience reactions to
the written pr
oduct. The students write and revise several
compositions. The teacher makes comments on each draft of
the composition with the intention of incr
easing specificity, focus,
and impact of the writing.
Source: Hillocks, 1982
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
20
Explicit teacher training was a major factor in the success of the process writing approach.When
teachers had such training, the effect was moderate (0.46), but in the absence of training the effect
was negligible, except for students in grades four to six, where the effect size was small (0.27) but
significant. Five of the six studies in which teachers received training in applying the process writing
model were conducted by the National Writing Project (NWP)
to provide support for its work. Additional research is needed to
verify these findings, particularly as the content of NWP training
has changed over time.Also, it was not always clear what teachers
learned or subsequently applied in their classrooms in the NWP
studies; random assignment did not occur in any of the NWP
studies; NWP was a partner in much of this research; and in some
instances the NWP teachers were volunteers. Nevertheless, it is
interesting to note that many of the components included in a
recent description of the NWP model (peers working together, inquiry, and sentence-combining; see
Nagin, 2003) were found by this meta-analysis to enhance the quality of adolescents’ writing.
Study of Models (Effect Size = 0.25)
The study of models provides
adolescents with good models
for each type of writing that is
the focus of instruction.
Students are encouraged to
analyze these examples and to
emulate the critical elements,
patterns, and forms embodied
in the models in their own
writing.The effects for all six
studies reviewed were positive, though small. It was not possible to draw separate conclusions for
low-achieving writers, as none of the studies specifically addressed this population.
Writing for Content Area Learning (Effect Size = 0.23)
Writing has been shown to be an effective tool for enhancing students’ learning of content material.
Although the impact of writing activity on content learning is small, it is consistent enough to predict
some enhancement in learning as a result of writing-to-learn activities.
About 75% of the wr
iting-to-lear
n studies analyzed had positive effects.The average effect was small
but significant.
Unfortunately, it was not possible to draw separate conclusions for low-achieving
writers, as none of the studies examined the impact of writing-to-learn activities specifically with
The process writing
approach
stresses activities
that emphasize extended
opportunities for writing,
writing for real audiences,
self-reflection, personalized
instruction and goals,
and cycles of planning,
translating, and reviewing.
STUDY OF MODELS: AN EXAMPLE
An example of Study of Models involves presenting students
with two models of excellent writing, such as a well-written essay
that sets out to persuade the reader that UFOs exist and another
well-written persuasive essay claiming that there is no such thing
as a UFO. The teacher discusses the essays with the students.
The next day, students are given the essay that claimed that
UFOs do not exist and are asked to write a persuasive essay
arguing for or against the position that girls are not better in
math than are boys.
Sour
ce: Knudson, 1991
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
21
these students.Writing-to-learn
was equally effective for all
content areas (social studies,
math, and science) and grades
(4–6 versus 7–12) studied.
A Note About Grammar
Instruction
Grammar instruction in the
studies reviewed involved the
explicit and systematic teaching
of the parts of speech and
structure of sentences.The
meta-analysis found an effect for this type of instruction for students across the full range of ability, but
surprisingly, this effect was negative.This negative effect was small, but it was statistically significant,
indicating that traditional g
rammar instr
uction is unlikely to help improve the quality of students’
writing.
Studies specifically examining the impact of grammar instruction with lo
w-achie
ving writers
also yielded negative results (Anderson,
1997;
Saddler & Graham, 2005). Such findings raise serious
questions about some educators’
enthusiasm for traditional g
rammar instruction as a focus of writing
instruction for adolescents. Ho
wever, other instructional methods, such as sentence combining, provide
an effective alter
native to traditional grammar instruction, as this approach improves students’ writing
quality while at the same time enhancing syntactic skills.
In addition, a recent study (F
earn & Farnan,
2005) found that teaching students to focus on the function and practical application of g
rammar
within the context of writing (versus teaching grammar as an independent activity) produced strong
and positive effects on students’ writing. Overall, the findings on grammar instruction suggest that,
although teaching grammar is important, alternative procedures, such as sentence combining, are more
effective than traditional approaches for improving the quality of students’ writing.
WRITING-TO-LEARN: AN EXAMPLE
In a science class, the students study the human circulatory
system. The teacher’s goal is to help students develop alternative
conceptualizations of the role of the heart, blood, and circulation.
The science teacher asks the students to write summaries and
answer questions in writing to increase their ability to explain
information, elaborate knowledge leading to deeper understanding
of the topic, comment on and interpret information in the written
science text, communicate what has not been understood, and
describe a change of belief they might be experiencing. Note
that in the writing-to-learn approach, the teacher assigns writing
tasks but does not provide explicit instruction in writing skills.
Thus, writing is a tool of lear
ning content material rather than an
end in itself.
Source: Boscolo & Mason, 2001
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
22
IMPLEMENTING THE ELEMENTS
Although currently eclipsed by reading instruction, teaching students to write played a central role in
education in many historical periods, from ancient Greek times through the 19th century. Indeed,
writing well was once a central goal of education in both Europe and the United States. However,
what is valued in writing, and thus the focus of writing instruction, has changed over the years. For
example, while the Greeks prized rhetoric, or persuasive writing, the Romans prized eloquence. In
the 18th- and 19th-century British Empire, the teaching of writing was seen as an important way to
instill moral values. Although writing instruction played a prominent role in U.S. schools during the
19th century, by the 20th century it was already on the wane (Murphy, 1990). It is possible, though,
that writing instruction will regain some of its earlier prominence as a result of the acknowledgement
that writing difficulties are occurring across the nation.
Aims of
Writing Instruction
Moder
n writing instruction in the United States recognizes that students need to write clearly and
for a wide v
ariety of real-life purposes.Thus,
flexibility is no
w perhaps the most prized goal of writing
instruction because the fully proficient writer can adapt to different contexts, formats, and purposes
for writing.
Most contexts of life (school, the workplace, and the community) call for writing skills, and each
context makes overlapping but not identical demands. Proficient writers can adapt their writing to
its context.Writing is also produced in different formats, such as sentences, lists, outlines, paragraphs,
essays, letters, and books. Proficient writers can flexibly move among most, if not all, of these formats.
Proficient writers are also able to move among purposes that range from writing solely for themselves
(as in a per
sonal diar
y) to comm
unicating with an external audience.
Wr
iting in School
Despite the r
eal-w
orld need for flexibility in wr
iting skill,
classr
oom instruction sometimes over-
emphasizes cer
tain for
ms of wr
iting o
ver others. For example, many students are taught a specific
and quite r
ig
id str
uctur
e for writing an essay, commonly known as the “five-paragraph essay. Skilled
wr
iter
s,
ho
wever, have more than that single structure to draw on when approaching a writing task.
The
y ha
v
e a v
ariety of forms, strategies, knowledge, and skills at their disposal that they can apply
flexib
ly to achie
v
e their wr
iting goals. Further, some teachers may overemphasize correct grammar or
spelling at the expense of the expr
ession of ideas.
Excellent instr
uction in wr
iting not only emphasizes
correctness of for
ms and conventions, but also instills in writers the command of a wide variety of
forms, genr
es, styles, and tones, and the ability to adapt to different contexts and purposes.
The use of different genres deserves special mention. Research has shown that, to the extent
that teacher
s r
equir
e wr
iting in the early g
rades,
the
y tend mainly to ask students to write stories,
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
23
descriptions of personal experiences, and other kinds of narratives. However, in the middle and high
school years, writing assignments typically involve expository tasks, such as reporting, summarizing and
analyzing factual information, and expressing an opinion with the support of evidence. Sixty percent
of writing assignments in 4th grade, 65% in 8th grade, and 75% in 12th grade are expository in nature
(Persky et al., 2003). Moreover, expository writing is the most frequently assigned writing task at the
college level (Bridgeman & Carlson, 1984).
Writing plays two distinct roles in school. First, it is a skill that draws on subskills and processes such as
handwriting and spelling; a rich knowledge of vocabulary; mastery of the conventions of punctuation,
capitalization, word usage, and grammar; and the use of strategies (such as planning, evaluating, and
revising text). All are necessary for the production of coherently organized essays containing well-
developed and pertinent ideas, supporting examples, and appropriate detail (Needels & Knapp, 1994).
This role can be characterized as “learning to write. Second, writing is a means to extend and deepen
students’ knowledge; it acts as a tool for learning subject matter (Keys, 2000; Shanahan, 2004; Sperling
& Freedman, 2001).This role is called “writing to learn. In fact, the roles of learning to write and
of writing to learn are interdependent. It was for this reason that Biancarosa and Snow (2004)
recommended that language arts teachers use content-area texts to teach reading and writing skills
and that content-area teachers provide instruction and practice in discipline-specific reading and
writing. Using writing tasks to learn content offers students opportunities to expand their knowledge
of vocabulary; to strengthen the planning, evaluating, and revising process; and to practice grammar,
spelling, punctuation, modes of argumentation,
and technical writing (Yore, 2003).
Wr
iting De
velops
Wr
iting proficiency develops over time. It begins as a kind of free association of ideas that a reader
ma
y find difficult to follow. From this comes a growing knowledge of stylistic conventions and more
sophisticated uses of pr
ocesses for planning,
e
v
aluating, and revising. Development continues with
compositions mark
ed b
y a
w
areness of an audience and writing as a more unified and productive
craft.
Finally
,
at the most adv
anced stage, writing becomes a personal tool for transforming one’s
o
wn exper
iences and kno
wledge (Bereiter, 1980). As they become more proficient writers, students
mo
v
e g
radually from “knowledge-telling” to “knowledge-transformation” (Bereiter & Scardamalia,
1987,
pp
.
5–6). Knowledge-telling is most typical of less proficient writers and involves writing
content that could in pr
inciple also be con
v
eyed orally. Knowledge-transformation is more
complex;
the wr
iting pr
ocess is used as a way to extend ideas and reasoning and as a vehicle for
the de
v
elopment of kno
wledge, philosophical ideas, and personal awareness.
Effectiv
e wr
iting instr
uction ackno
wledges that the smooth deplo
yment of the higher-level writing
strategies needed to plan,
generate
,
and r
e
vise text depends on easy use of lower-level skills such as
handwr
iting,
k
e
yboar
ding,
spelling, grammar and punctuation, and access to appropriate vocabulary.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
24
It will be harder for students to utilize strategies to write a coherent summary or persuasive essay if
they are not fluent in the lower-level skills. At the same time, students who have difficulty with either
lower-level writing skills or higher-level writing strategies will find it difficult to write to learn.
In-depth research performed with proficient adult writers has revealed important information about
the mental activity that underlies the act of writing (Flower, 1979; Flower & Hayes, 1981; Graham,
2006).This work has shaped the design of contemporary writing instruction—for example, the
planning, drafting, and revision process taught within the National Writing Project model (Pritchard &
Marshall, 1994). According to this model, writing is made up of closely linked processes that operate
simultaneously, as teachers model and guide students through various writing strategies.This research
framework also emphasizes building the motivation of low-achieving writers, which has often gone
into a steep decline by the time they are identified as needing help (Bruning & Horn, 2000; Garcia &
de Caso, 2004).
Choosing Elements for Implementation
This report identifies elements of instruction that hold promise for improving writing ability.
However, as mentioned earlier, together the instructional elements do not represent a curriculum.
Before implementing any of the elements, educators should consider the needs of their students as
revealed by assessment data. Such data include observations of students while they are writing, analysis
of their writing samples (see, e.g., Needels & Knapp, 1994), and test scores. Just as with reading,
writing intervention is most effective when matched to student needs. Once an intervention begins,
assessment and diagnostic teaching should be used in an ongoing w
ay to examine its effects (Barr,
Blacho
wicz,
Bates, Katz, & Kaufman, 2007). Not all elements are effective with all students and
all teacher
s. Sometimes positive results are not seen immediately; implementing new elements of
instr
uction often requires a significant investment of time to reveal their full potential (Graham &
Har
r
is,
2005).
Moreover, the elements identified in this report have not been jointly tested or
methodically compar
ed with each other
.
P
ending conclusive data on the relative effects of one
strategy o
v
er another
,
teachers should choose on the basis of fit with existing instructional conditions
and futur
e goals.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
25
ACCUMULATION OF RESEARCH ON
ADOLESCENT WRITING INSTRUCTION
This report is intended to inform the public about empirical evidence that has accumulated over
the years on what elements of writing instruction work best with adolescents.The last comprehensive
review of writing was conducted 20 years ago (Hillocks, 1986). In the intervening years, researchers
have focused their attention more narrowly, using meta-analysis to examine the impact of elements
such as strategy instruction (Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003), word processing (Bangert-
Drowns, 1993; Goldberg et al., 2003), and writing to learn (Bangert-Drowns et al., 2004). Studies
from these reviews were included in the current review, as were new investigations uncovered through
an extensive and rigorous search process.
A considerable body of research has accumulated since Hillocks
(1986). Although there is much less research on writing than
reading instruction, the current study located a total of 176 effect
sizes, only 29 of which came from the Hillocks review.These
effect sizes encompassed more than 25 categories of adolescent
writing instruction, with four or more effect sizes available for
18 of the categories (for seven of these categories, the findings
were not reliable enough or the instructional procedures too
diverse to be included here).Twenty-six writing-to-learn studies
with students in grades 4 to 12 were found, but only three of
these (Boscolo & Mason, 2001; Hand, Hohenshell, & Prain, 2004;
Wong, Kuperis, Jamieson, Keller, & Cull-Hewitt, 2002) were conducted after a writing-to-learn
meta-analysis that surveyed the research available through 1999 (Bangert-Drowns et al., 2004).Thanks
to all of this research, there is a reasonable body of experimental and quasi-experimental research from
which policymakers and educators can draw conclusions and develop recommendations.
The field of wr
iting r
esear
ch has matured in the past 20 years. Unfortunately, the number of studies
conducted with lo
w-achie
ving wr
iters, especially those from low-income families in inner-city
settings,
r
emains r
elatively small. Only 41 (23%) of the 176 effect sizes in this meta-analysis involved
lo
w-achie
ving wr
iters in general or students with learning disabilities, low English language proficiency,
or mild handicapping conditions.
Although the cur
r
ent study did not examine whether effect sizes
w
er
e moderated b
y the location of the study (i.e., urban, suburban, rural), there was a notable lack
of exper
imental r
esearch with low-achieving writers in urban settings. So, even though there is
an impr
essi
ve amount of research testing different approaches to writing instruction, the lack of
information on effecti
ve writing instruction for low-income, urban, low-achieving adolescent
writers remains a serious gap in the literature.
BURGEONING
RESEARCH BASE
In 1986, Hillocks was able
to calculate only 29 effect
sizes for students in grades
4 to 12 from existing
quasi-experimental and
experimental studies of
writing instruction. In 2005,
176 were located. The writing
instruction research base
has grown more than 600
percent in just 20 years.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
26
Identifying what combination or how much of each of the recommended elements is needed to
maximize writing instruction, for adolescents in general or for low-achieving writers in particular,
was beyond the scope of this study. Also, because studies of many aspects of writing instruction
remain to be done, the recommendations in this report do not address certain aspects of writing,
such as teaching spelling, handwriting, punctuation, and vocabulary, or certain approaches to
instruction, such as conferencing with students about their writing.The findings of the meta-analysis
do not provide clear direction for the use of technological tools other than word processing, nor is
there enough evidence to offer firm guidelines for how teachers can boost adolescents’ motivation to
write (c.f., Bruning & Horn, 2000; Garcia & de Caso, 2004).The conclusions of this report are based
only on rigorous empirical data; the areas for which the report has no recommendations point to gaps
in the current state of research on writing instruction.
A Research Agenda for Writing Instruction
Some instructional procedures have been investigated more often than others. Only four key elements
in this report’s list (strategy instruction, word processing, process writing approach, and writing-to-
learn activities) yielded 10 or more studies that met the criteria established for selection for the
meta-analysis. One other area of writing instruction—traditional grammar instruction—was the
focus of more than 10 studies, but its effects were negative. Some other areas of instruction showed
promise, but reliable conclusions could not be drawn because of limited evidence. In fact, some
recommendations from previous reports have an uneven research record. For instance, increasing
the frequency and amount of writing is generally proposed as an important practice because of
widespread concerns about how little actual writing students do in schools (Applebee, 2000; National
Commission on Writing, 2003), but evidence of a consistent effect is ambiguous.There were too few
effect sizes, too much variability in effect sizes, too much diversity in the procedures used to promote
extra writing time, and too many different comparison conditions to allow any reliable conclusions
to be drawn about the impact of this approach. As was claimed many years ago (Braddock & Jones,
1969), it may well be that although it is important for students to engage frequently in writing
practice, providing more opportunities to write without effective instruction and motivation is not
enough to improve writing quality.
Although wr
iting instr
uction has been r
esearched much less often than reading instruction, it is an
equally impor
tant component of literacy pr
oficiency and encompasses,
in itself, an extremely complex
set of skills.
The r
ich natur
e of the practice of writing and its relative neglect in instructional research
mak
e it ine
vitab
le that a whole compendium of possible approaches has not yet been studied.
Research is clearly needed not only to identify additional effecti
ve practices that already exist but to
develop new ones.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
27
Several rather well-established practices still have small and uneven research bases.Text structure
instruction, which involves explicitly and systematically teaching students about the structure of
specific types of text, such as stories, informational text, and persuasive essays, has not been studied
sufficiently. Another area in which more empirical evidence is needed is feedback, which involves
giving students structured responses about their writing.This study identified five studies that
examined the impact of feedback on the quality of students’ writing, but the methods and outcomes
were too variable to draw any reliable conclusions. In addition, a wide variety of external supports
(such as prompts, guides, hints, and visual frameworks that structure the writing process) designed
to facilitate students’ writing are in use in classrooms but have not been studied adequately, so firm
evidence for their efficacy is lacking. Further, as in the field of reading (Biancarosa & Snow, 2004),
more attention needs to be directed at the impact of individualized instruction as well as small-group
instruction, especially with low-achieving writers.
New practices in writing instruction have recently arisen and require investigation. For example,
teachers have begun to use rubrics to teach students to reflect on and evaluate the quality of their
own writing.The leveled lists of writing qualities or traits provided in a rubric give students tangible
evaluation criteria.This approach is gaining support among educators.Vocabulary instruction in
the context of writing is another practice that has been increasing recently and may be an effective
procedure for improving students’ writing.
When considered against a backdrop of the vast number of studies of reading instruction, research
into writing instruction needs more attention (National Commission on Writing, 2003).The coming
years must bring increased effort to collect stringent data on approaches to writing instruction that are
used in classrooms but for which evidence is lacking.The list of writing elements offered in this report
is limited by what has been studied rigorously thus far.Those who conduct the next meta-analysis
of writing instruction, whether 5, 10, or 20 years from now, need a body of literature to review that
scrutinizes a greater variety of instructional practices.
New researchers must take on the challenge of studying writing instruction in all its complexity.
Reading research was once limited in much the same way as writing research now is, but consistent
attention from the academic community brought forth a flood of knowledge about many aspects
of the reading process.Writing must be next.The future success of the nation’s young people
depends on it.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
28
A CHALLENGE
The results of this meta-analysis point toward a number of specific changes that teachers can make
to promote increased writing proficiency among all their students. One important outcome of
improved writing is students’ ability to use it as a tool for understanding and analyzing subject-matter
information. Setting high standards through implementing effective writing instruction sends
adolescents a message that higher-level thinking about substantive material is important. In the
words of an adolescent attending an innovative inner-city high school in New York City, “They
wanted, like, essays.They wanted you to get it” (Herszenhorn, 2005).
Improving the writing abilities of adolescent students has social implications far beyond the classroom.
Helping these young people to write clearly, logically, and coherently about ideas, knowledge, and
views will expand their access to higher education, give them an edge for advancement in the
workforce, and increase the likelihood they will actively participate as citizens of a literate society.
Only the combined efforts of policymakers, educators, and researchers will make this happen.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
29
REFERENCES
Achieve, Inc. (2005). Rising to the challenge:Are high school graduates prepared for college and work?
Washington, DC: Author.
ACT. (2005).
Crisis at the core: Preparing all students for college and work. Iowa City:Author. Retrieved
July 31, 2006, from http://www.act.org/path/policy/pdf/crisis_report.pdf
ACT. (2006).
Reading between the lines:What the ACT reveals about college readiness in reading. Iowa City:
Author. Retrieved July 31, 2006, from http://www.act.org/path/policy/pdf/reading_report.pdf
American Diploma Project. (2004).
Ready or not: Creating a high school diploma that counts.Washington,
DC: Achieve, Inc.
Anderson, A. A. (1997).
The effects of sociocognitive writing strategy instruction on the writing achievement and
writing self-efficacy of students with disabilities and typical achievement in an urban elementary school
.
Unpub
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Houston, Houston,TX.
Anthony, H., & Anderson, L. (1987, January ).The nature of writing instruction in regular and special
education classrooms. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of American Educational Research
Association, San Francisco, CA.
Applebee, A. (1986). Problems in process approaches:Towards reconceptualization of process
instruction.
In A. Petrosky & D. Bartholomae (Eds.),
The teaching of wr
iting. Eighty-fifth Yearbook of the
National Society for the Study of Education, Part II (pp. 95–113). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Applebee, A. (2000). Alternative models of writing development. In R. Indrisano & J. Squire (Eds.),
Perspectives on writing research, theory, and practice (pp.90–110). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
Applebee, A., Langer, J., Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (2003). Discussion-based approaches to
developing understanding: Classroom instruction and student performance in middle and high
school English.
American Educational Research Journal, 40, 685–730.
Bangert-Dr
owns, R. (1993).The word processor as an instructional tool: A meta-analysis of word
pr
ocessing in writing instruction.
Review of Educational Researc
h, 63,
69–93.
Bangert-Drowns, R. L., Hurley, M. M., & Wilkinson, B. (2004).The effects of school-based Writing-
to-Learn interventions on academic achievement: A meta-analysis.
Review of Educational Research,
74, 29–58.
Barr, R., Blachowicz, C. Z., Bates, A., Katz, C., & Kaufman, B. (2007).
Reading diagnosis for teachers: An
instructional approac
h
(5th ed).
Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Bean,T.W., & Steenwyk, F. L. (1984).The effect of three forms of summarization instruction on sixth
graders’ summary writing and comprehension.
Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 297–306.
Bereiter, C. (1980). Development in writing. In L.W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.),
Cognitive processes
in wr
iting
.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987).
The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Biancarosa, G., & Snow, C. E. (2004).
Reading next: A vision for action and research in middle and high school
literacy:A report from the Carnegie Corporation of New York.
Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent
Education.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
30
Boscolo, P., & Mason, L. (2001).Writing to learn, writing to transfer. In G. Rijlaarsdam, P.Tynjala, L.
Mason, & K. Lonka (Eds.), Studies in writing:Vol. 7.Writing as a learning tool: Integrating theory and
practice
(pp. 83–104). Dordrecht,The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Braddock, R., & Jones, R. L. (1969). English composition. In R. L. Ebel (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of
educational research (4th ed., pp. 443–461). New York: Macmillan.
Bridgeman, B., & Carlson, S.B. (1984). Survey of academic writing tasks.
Written Communication, 1,
247–280.
Bruning, R., & Horn, C. (2000). Developing motivation to write.
Educational Psychologist, 35, 25–37.
Calkins, L. (1981). Case study of a nine year old writer. In D. Graves (Ed.),
A case study observing
development of primary-children’s composing, spelling, and motor behavior during the writing process
(pp. 239–262). Durham: University of New Hampshire.
Carnevale,A. P. (2001).
Help wanted… college required.Washington, DC: Educational Testing Service,
Office for Pub
lic Leadership.
Chafe,W., & Tannen, D. (1987).The relation between written and spoken language.
Annual Review of
Anthropology, 16,
383–407.
Curry, K. A. (1997).
A comparison of the writing products of students with learning disabilities in inclusive and
resource room settings using different writing instruction approaches. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL.
Dailey, E. M. (1991).
The relative efficacy of cooperative learning versus individualized learning on the written
performance of adolescent students with writing problems. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, John Hopkins
University, Baltimore, MD.
De La Paz, S. (1999). Self-regulated strategy instruction in regular education settings: Improving
outcomes for students with and without learning disabilities.
Learning Disabilities Research and
Practice, 14, 92–106.
De La Paz,
S., & Graham, S. (2002). Explicitly teaching strategies, skills, and knowledge:Writing
instruction in middle school classrooms.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 291–304.
Duin,
A. H., & Graves, M. F. (1987). Intensive vocabulary instruction as a prewriting technique.
Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 311–330.
Fearn, L., & Farnan, N. (2005, April).
An investigation of the influence of teaching grammar in writing to
accomplish an influence on wr
iting
.
P
aper pr
esented at the ann
ual meeting of the
Amer
ican Educational
Research Association, Montreal, Canada.
Ferretti, R. P., MacArthur, C. A., & Dowdy, N. S. (2000).The effects of an elaborated goal on the
persuasive writing of students with learning disabilities and their normally achieving peers.
Journal
of Educational Psychology, 92, 694–702.
Fitzgerald, J., & Markham, L. (1987).Teaching children about revision in writing.
Cognition and
Instruction, 4, 3–24.
Fitzgerald, J., & Shanahan,T. (2000). Reading and writing relations and their development.
Educational
Psychologist, 35, 39–50.
Flower, L. (1979).Writer-based prose:A cognitive basis for problems in writing.
College English, 41,
19–37.
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing.
College Composition &
Communication,
32,
365–387.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
31
Garcia, J. N., & de Caso, A. M. (2004). Effects of a motivational intervention for improving the writing
of children with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 27, 141–159.
Goldberg, A., Russell, M., & Cook, A. (2003).The effects of computers on student writing: A
metaanalysis of studies from 1992 to 2002.
Journal of Technology, Learning, and Assessment, 2, 1–51.
Golden, P. (1986).Writing process reconsidered.
Boston Writing Project Newsletter.
Graham, S. (1990).The role of production factors in Learning Disabled students’ compositions.
Journal
of Educational Psychology, 82,
781–791.
Graham, S. (2006). Strategy instruction and the teaching of writing: A meta-analysis. In C. MacArthur,
S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.),
Handbook of writing research (pp. 187–207). New York: Guilford.
Graham, S. (2006).Writing. In P. Alexander & P.Winne (Eds.),
Handbook of educational psychology
(pp. 457–478). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Graham. S., & Harris, K. R. (2003). Students with learning disabilities and the process of writing:
A meta-analysis of SRSD studies.
In L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.),
Handbook of
research on learning disabilities (pp. 383–402). New York: Guilford.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2005).
Writing better. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Graham,
S., & Perin, D. (2006).
A meta-anal
ysis of writing instruction for adolescent students
. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Greene, J. P., & Winters, M. A. (2005).
Public high school graduation and college-readiness rates: 1991-2002
(Education Working Paper No. 8). New York: Manhattan Institute. Retrieved July 31, 2006, from
http://www.manhattan-institute.org/pdf/ewp_08.pdf
Gregg, L., & Steinberg, E. (Eds.). (1980).
Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grubb,W. N.,Worthen, H., Byrd, B.,Webb, E., Badway, N., Case, C., et al. (1999).
Honored but invisible:
An inside look at teaching in community colleges. New York: Routledge.
Hand,
B., Hohenshell, L., & Prain,V. (2004). Exploring students’ responses to conceptual questions
when engaged with planned writing experiences: A study with year ten science students.
Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 41, 186–210.
Har
r
is, K., & Graham, S. (1996).
Making the wr
iting process work:
Strategies for composition and self-regulation
(2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books.
Har
r
is, K. R., & Graham, S. (1999). Programmatic intervention research: Illustrations from the
evolution of self-regulated strategy development.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 251–262.
Hayes, J. R. (2000). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In R.
Indrisano & J. R. Squire (Eds.),
Perspectives on writing: Research, theory and practice (pp. 6-44). Newark,
DE:
International Reading Association. Originally published in C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.)
(1996).
The science of writing:Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 1–27).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hedges,
L.,
& Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Herszenhorn, D. M. (2005, August 08). In New York’s smaller schools, ‘good year and a tough year.
New York Times, p. A1.
Hillocks, G., Jr. (1982).The interaction of instruction, teacher comment, and revision in teaching the
composing process.
Research in the Teaching of English, 16, 261–278.
Hillocks,
G
.
(1986).
Researc
h on wr
itten composition:
New directions for teaching
.
Urbana,
IL:
National
Council of Teachers of English.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
32
Kamil, M. L. (2003). Adolescents and literacy: Reading for the 21st century.Washington, DC: Alliance for
Excellent Education.
Kerchner, L. B., & Kistinger, B. J. (1984). Language processing/word processing:Written expression,
computers and learning disabled students.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 7, 329–335.
Keys, C.W. (2000). Investigating the thinking processes of eighth grade writers during the composition
of a scientific laboratory report.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 676–690.
Knudson, R. E. (1989). Effects of instructional strategies on children’s informational writing.
Journal of
Educational Research, 83,
91–96.
Knudson, R. E. (1991). Effects of instructional strategies, grade, and sex on students’ persuasive writing.
Journal of Experimental Education, 59, 141–152.
Langer, J. A., & Applebee, A. (1987).
How writing shapes thinking: Studies of teaching and learning. (NCTE
Research Report 22). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Lemk
e, M., Sen, A., Pahlke, E., Partelow, L., Miller, D.,Williams,T., et al. (2004).
Inter
national outcomes of
learning in mathematics, literacy, and problem solving: PISA 2003 results from the U.S. perspective
. (NCES
2005-003).Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education
Statistics.
Levy, M., & Ransdell, S. (1996).
The science of writing:Theories, methods, individual differences, and
applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lipsey, M., & Wilson, D. (2001).
Practical meta-analysis.Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
MacArthur, C., Graham, S., & Fitzgerald, J. (2006).
Handbook of writing research. New York: Guilford.
MacArthur, C., Schwartz, S., & Graham, S. (1991). Effects of a reciprocal peer revision strategy in
special education classrooms.
Learning Disability Research and Pr
actice, 6,
201–210.
Murphy, J. J. (Ed.). (1990).
A short history of writing instruction from ancient Greece to twentieth-century
America. Davis, CA: Hermagoras Press.
Nagin, C. (2003).
Because writing matters: Improving student writing in our schools. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
National Center for Education Statistics.
(1999).
Nation’
s repor
t card: Reading 1998
.
W
ashington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved July 31, 2006, from
http://nces.ed.go
v/pubsear
ch/pubsinfo
.asp?pubid=1999500
National Center for Education Statistics. (2003, November).
Remedial education at degree-granting
postsecondary institutions in Fall 2000: Statistical analysis report
. (Technical Report, NCES 2004-0101).
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education Institute of Education Science.
National Center for Education Statistics.
(2005).
A first look at the liter
acy of America’s adults in the 21st
century
.Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved July 31, 2006, from
http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2006470
National Center for Education Statistics.
(2006).
The condition of education 2006.
W
ashington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved July 31, 2006, from
http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2006071
National Commission on
Wr
iting.
(2003,
April).
The neglected R:
The need for a wr
iting rev
olution
.
Retrieved July 31, 2006, from http://www.writingcommission.org/report.html
National Commission on
Wr
iting.
(2004,
September).
Wr
iting:
A tic
ket to work… or a tic
ket out:
A sur
vey
of business leaders
.
Retr
ie
v
ed July 31, 2006, from http://www.writingcommission.org/report/html
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
33
National Commission on Writing. (2005, July). Writing:A powerful message from state government.
Retrieved July 31, 2006, from http://www.writingcommission.org/report/html
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000).
Report of the National Reading
Panel.Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and
its implications for reading instruction
(Publication No. 00-4769).Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Needels, M. C., & Knapp, M. S. (1994).Teaching writing to children who are underserved.
Journal of
Educational Psychology, 86,
339–349.
OECD (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development). (2000).
Literacy in the information
age: Final report of the international adult literacy survey
. Paris, France: Author. Retrieved July 31, 2006,
from http://www1.oecd.org/publications/e-book/8100051E.pdf
Olson,
L. (2006). A decade of effort.
Quality Counts
, 25,
8–10,
12, 14, 16, 18–21.
P
erin, D. (2006). Can community colleges protect both access and standards? The problem of
remediation.
Teachers College Record, 108, 339–373.
Perin, D., & Charron, K. (2006).“Lights just click on every day”: Academic preparedness and
remediation in comm
unity colleges. In
T. R. Bailey & V. S. Morest (Eds.),
Defending the comm
unity
college equity agenda
. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
Persky, H. R., Daane, M. C., & Jin,Y. (2003).
The nation’s report card:Writing 2002. (NCES 2003–529).
U.S. Depar
tment of Education. Institute of Education Sciences. National Center for Education
Statistics.Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
Pinkus, Lyndsay. (2006).
Who’s counted? Who’s counting? Understanding high school graduation rates.
Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.
Pressley, M., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2006).The state of educational intervention research.
British
Journal of Educational Psycholog
y, 76,
1–19.
Pressle
y, M.,Yokoi, L., Rankin, J.,Wharton-McDonald, R., & Mistretta, J. (1997). A survey of the
instructional practices of grade 5 teachers nominated as effective in promoting literacy.
Scientific
Studies of Reading, 1,
1–16.
Prior, P. (2006). A sociocultural theory of writing. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.),
Handbook of wr
iting researc
h
(pp
.
54–66).
New York: Guilford.
Pr
itchar
d,
R.
J
.
(1987).
Effects on student wr
iting of teacher training in the National Writing Project
Model.
Written Communication, 4, 51–67.
Pritchard, R. J., & Honeycutt, J. (2006). Process writing. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald
(Eds.),
Handbook of writing research (pp. 275–290). New York: Guilford.
Pr
itchar
d,
R.
J., & Marshall, J. C. (1994). Evaluation of a tiered model for staff development in writing.
Research in the Teaching of English, 28, 259–285.
Rosenberg,
M.,
Adams,
D
.,
& Gur
evitch, J. (2000).
MetaWin:
Statistical soft-w
are for meta-anal
ysis
(Version 2). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Saddler, B., & Graham, S. (2005).The effects of peer-assisted sentence-combining instruction on the
writing performance of more and less skilled young writers.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
43–54.
Shanahan,T. (2004). Overcoming the dominance of communication:Writing to think and to learn. In
T. L.
J
etton & J
.
A.
Dole (Eds.).
Adolescent liter
acy researc
h and pr
actice
(pp
.
59–73).
Ne
w
Y
ork:
Guilfor
d.
Smagorinsky, P. (Ed.). (2006).
Research on composition. New York:Teachers College.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
34
Snow, C. E., & Biancarosa, G. (2003). Adolescent literacy and the achievement gap:What do we know and
where do we go from here? New York: Carnegie Corporation of New York.
Sperling, M., & Freedman, S.W. (2001). Review of writing research. In V. Richardson (Ed.),
Handbook
of research on teaching
(4th ed., pp. 370–389).Washington, DC: American Educational Research
Association.
Stotsky, S. G. (1988). Commentary.
Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 89–99.
Troia, G., & Graham, S. (2002).The effectiveness of a highly explicit, teacher-directed strategy
instruction routine: Changing the writing performance of students with learning disabilities.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 290–305.
Wolf, I. (1986).
Meta-analysis: Quantitative methods for research synthesis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Wong, B.Y. L., Kuperis, S., Jamieson, D., Keller, L., & Cull-Hewitt, R. (2002). Effects of guided journal
writing on students’ story understanding.
Journal of Educational Research, 95, 179–191.
Y
arrow, F., & Topping, K. J. (2001). Collaborative writing:The effects of metacognitive prompting and
structured peer interaction.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 261–282.
Yeh, S. (1998). Empowering education:Teaching argumentative writing to cultural minority middle-
school students.
Research in the Teaching of English, 33, 49–83.
Yore, L. D. (2003). Examining the literacy component of science literacy: 25 years of language arts and
science resear
ch.
International Jour
nal of Science Education, 25,
689–725.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
35
APPENDIX A: META-ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY
This appendix reviews in more detail the methodology in conducting the meta-analysis that yielded
the
Writing Next recommendations. A more in-depth description of the methodology can be found in
a forthcoming publication (Graham & Perin, under review).
Location and Selection of Studies
This meta-analysis was performed to draw a series of instructional recommendations for teaching
writing as well as update the conclusions drawn by Bangert-Drowns, Hurley, and Wilkinson (2004)
about the effectiveness of writing as a tool for learning.The strategies used for locating and selecting
studies for inclusion were influenced by nine main factors.
First, the search concentrated on two separate areas: learning-to-write and writing-to-learn. For
learning-to-write, studies of the effectiveness of interventions designed to improve students’ writing
quality were looked for. For writing-to-learn, all relevant studies from Bangert-Drowns et al.s (2004)
meta-analysis were included and writing-to-learn studies conducted after 2000 were searched, since
Bangert-Drown et al.s (2004) search ended in 1999.
Second, the investigation was limited to studies of students in grades 4 to 12. Some studies that
included students in grades 4 to 12 were eliminated if they also included students in the primary
grades and it was not possible to calculate an effect size solely for the older students.
Third, studies of writing intervention in special schools for children with deafness, autism, or severe
emotional disturbance were not included.While writing instruction is an important part of the
curriculum for these students, the purpose of this review was to draw recommendations for writing
instruction within regular school settings.
F
our
th,
only studies that used a measur
e of wr
iting quality for the learning-to-write analysis were
included.
Wr
iting quality w
as the pr
imar
y outcome, or one of several primary outcomes, in all
pr
e
vious meta-analyses on wr
iting instr
uction (Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Goldberg, Russell, & Cook,
2003;
Graham,
2006;
Graham & Har
ris, 2003; Hillocks, 1986).The review was limited to studies of
wr
iting quality to identify inter
v
entions that had a br
oad impact on writing performance.The only
exception in
v
olv
ed summar
y-writing instruction. Measures of quality for studies in this category
w
er
e based on the completeness and accuracy of the wr
itten summar
y produced by students.
Fifth, studies in which reliability for the writing quality measure was questionable were excluded. For
example, studies that provided no information on interrater reliability or that provided no training to
raters were eliminated. Studies in which low levels of interrater reliability (i.e., below 0.60) were report-
ed were also eliminated.Almost all contemporary measures of writing quality involve some degree of
subjecti
vity
,
making the estab
lishment of r
eliability of scor
ing pr
ocedures particularly important.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
36
Sixth, writing-to-learn studies were eliminated if outcomes did not include an assessment of
content-related academic achievement in addition to writing quality measures.These content-
learning measures were conducted in the school setting where the investigation took place and
varied in form, including such diverse assessments as final grades, locally constructed examinations,
and standardized tests.
Seventh, only studies employing an experimental or quasi-experimental design were included.This
method was consistent with most previous meta-analysis in writing (Bangert-Drowns, 1993;
Bangert-Drowns et al., 2004; Goldberg et al., 2003; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003; Hillocks,
1986). Consequently, each study in this meta-analysis compared at least two groups of students who
received different instructional conditions. Correlational, qualitative, or single-subject design studies
and studies in which students served as their own comparison group were not included. Even
though each of these other types of research has much to add to the ongoing dialogue about effective
instructional practices (see Pressley, Graham, & Harris, 2006), the review was concerned specifically
with experimental and quasi-experimental studies.
Eighth, only studies that provided the data needed to calculate appropriate statistics, including an
effect size, a weighted average effect size, and homogeneity, were included. For example, if a study did
not provide information on the number of participants involved in the experiment (e.g., Kerchner &
Kristinger, 1984), it was excluded, as it could not be used in the calculation of homogeneity or an
average weighted effect size.
Ninth, a search that was as broad as possible was undertaken to identify relevant studies for both the
learning-to-write and the writing-to-learn analyses. Included in this search were studies with students
in grades 4 to 12 in previous meta-analyses of writing (i.e., Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Bangert-Drowns
et al., 2004; Goldberg et al., 2003; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003; Hillocks, 1986). Edited
books and book series were also searched for possible studies (e.g., Gregg & Steinberg, 1980; Levy &
Ransdell, 1996). Further, multiple searches in a number of databases, including ERIC, PsycINFO,
ProQuest, Education Abstracts (i.e., Education Full Text), and Dissertation Abstracts, were run during
May 2005 to identify relevant studies. Of 582 documents collected as well as the studies for grades
4 to 12 reported in Bangert-Drowns (1993), Bangert-Drowns et al. (2004), and Hillocks (1986), 142
studies were identified as suitable for inclusion. Some of these contained more than one intervention
comparison, and a total of 176 different effect sizes were generated from these studies.
Categorizing Studies Into Intervention Conditions
Each study was read and then placed into pre-identified groupings.While some of the pre-identified
groupings were developed from previous meta-analyses (Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Bangert-Drowns
et al., 2004; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003), others reflected the broad array of topics
included in the electronic searches described in the previous section. Studies that did not fit neatly
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
37
into groupings were held apart until all studies had been read once.Then, studies in each
pre-identified category were examined to determine if each investigation represented the same
basic intervention. If they did not, they were held apart with studies not categorized during the
initial reading.After all the groupings had been verified in this manner, uncategorized studies were
reread and new groupings were constructed as needed. In a few instances, a study was placed into
one of the pre-identified groupings. Studies in any new grouping as well as any pre-identified
grouping in which an additional study was placed were read again to determine if each intervention
represented the same basic approach. Some studies were placed into more than one grouping
because they included two or more interventions.
As noted previously the groupings fell into two general types: learning-to-write and writing-to-learn.
Writing-to-learn studies were not further categorized, but learning-to-write studies were. Learning-
to-write groupings fell into three broad categories (explicit instruction, instructional supports, and
mode of instruction). Because summary effect sizes were calculated only for groupings that contained
four or more effect sizes, only those groupings are described by category here (a list of all groupings
with four or more effect sizes is presented in Appendix B).
Explicit Instruction
A considerable number of studies focused on explicitly teaching skills, processes, or knowledge.
All these studies involved sustained, direct, and systematic instruction designed to facilitate student
mastery.Three categories—writing strategies, summarization, and sentence combining—yielded at
least four or mor
e effect sizes that pr
ovided a reliable estimate.
Strategy instruction shared some overlap with other categories classified as “explicit teaching.While
the primary focus was always on teaching planning, revising, and/or editing strategies, some studies
(especially those involving the Self-Regulated Strategy Development model; Harris & Graham, 1996)
also directly taught students knowledge and skills needed to use these processes. It is also important to
note that some author
s w
ould classify strategy instr
uction as a pr
ocess wr
iting appr
oach (see Pr
itchar
d
& Hone
ycutt,
2006),
as both ar
e concer
ned with pr
omoting students’
use of processes for planning,
translating,
and r
e
vie
wing.
Strategy instr
uction w
as not included under the process writing approach
for tw
o r
easons:
(a) explicit and sustained instr
uction in wr
iting strateg
ies is not common in man
y
classr
ooms that use a pr
ocess appr
oach model (e
.g.,
Anthon
y &
Anderson, 1987) and (b) such
instr
uction is rar
ely included in descr
iptions of the components of a pr
ocess wr
iting pr
o
gram (see, e.g.,
Nag
in,
2003).
Ev
en though sentence combining is included as a component of the National
Wr
iting
Project,
this also w
as not included under the pr
ocess wr
iting appr
oach, as it is only one of many
elements included in this model.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
38
I
nstructional Supports
Supporting students’ writing involves providing some form of assistance that helps them carry out
one or more processes involved in writing.These procedures include structuring how students carry
out a particular writing process, having peers help each other as they compose, providing students
with feedback on their performance, focusing students’ attention on specific aspects of the task, and
providing a model of what the end product should look like. Six supporting categories—collaborative
writing, specific product goals, word processing, prewriting, inquiry activities, and study of models—
yielded four or more effect sizes that provided a reliable estimate.
These procedures differ from the categories included under “explicit teaching” in a subtle but
important way.The primary goal for explicit instruction categories, such as strategy instruction and
sentence combining, is the immediate mastery of the declarative and procedural knowledge targeted
for instruction.This goal is accomplished by directly teaching such knowledge until students can apply
it independently.The mastery of processes, knowledge, or skills is more indirect with the supporting
procedures described above, as students are assumed to internalize the use of these procedures as a
result of applying them over time. For example, as students repeatedly analyze models of good writing
and attempt to emulate them, it is assumed that they develop a better understanding of the criteria
underlying good writing and that they increasingly begin to apply this newly acquired knowledge
without having to rely on the models for assistance.
It is also important to realize that some of these supporting procedures are integral components
of explicit teaching procedures. For example, with strategy instruction, students often help each other
apply (and adapt) a strategy as they are learning to use it, they are encouraged to attain specific goals
for their paper
s that can be achieved by using the strategy, and they receive feedback on how the
strategy has enhanced their writing (Graham & Harris, 2003).These supporting procedures are not
elements just of explicit teaching procedures, however, as many of them (e.g., prewriting and inquiry
activities, feedback, and peers working together to compose a composition) are integral to the process
approach to writing as well. Finally, it is noted that inquiry could be classified as a prewriting activity.
However, it was decided to keep it as a separate category because it employs a set of distinctive
features for developing and analyzing ideas, and it was also kept distinct in Hillocks’ (1986) review
(as was the study of models).
Mode of Instruction
In the meta-analysis conducted by Hillocks (1986), four categories (presentational, environmental,
individualized, and natural process) encompassed teachers’ mode or form of instruction.These modes
differed in the role assumed by the teacher, the kinds and order of activities applied, and the specificity
and clarity of objectives and learning tasks.The
presentational mode was not examined, as there were
too few studies. In addition, the
environmental mode was not investigated, as this category has been
criticized in terms of its distinctiveness and clarity (Applebee, 1986; Golden, 1986; Stotsky, 1988), and
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
39
most of the studies included in this category could also be placed in the inquiry activities category
too.The
individualized mode of instruction was also not included in this investigation, because the
interventions included under this category were too diverse to form a cohesive treatment.
For the meta-analysis, the more common title
process writing approach was adopted for what Hillocks
called the
natural process mode. Included under the “process writing” umbrella were studies that
examined the effectiveness of the process writing approach as described by Graves (1983), the Writers’
Workshop (Calkins, 1981), the National Writing Project (Pritchard, 1987), and whole language
(when a process writing approach was applied).
Coding of Study Features
Each study was coded for seven variables: grade, type of student, number of participants, writing genre,
assignment of subjects, comparison condition, and publication type.These variables provided informa-
tion on who received an intervention (grade and type of student), how broadly it was applied (number
of participants), what genre it was designed to impact (writing genre), and what intervention served
as the comparison condition. Most of these variables were also selected because it was assumed that
they might account for variability in effect sizes beyond subject-level sampling error (assessed by the
test of homogeneity). For example, variability in effect sizes may be related to systematic differences
in the comparison condition, type or grade level of participants, and writing genre assessed.To
establish reliability of the coding procedures used in this review, a doctoral student majoring in
education was familiar
ized with each category and then asked to score 15% of the studies (randomly
selected).
Acr
oss studies and variables, interrater reliability between the graduate student and our
coding w
as 94%.
Two of the study feature variables are described below because they are included in
Appendix B
. See Graham and Perin (2006) for a fuller discussion of these and other features.
Grade.The specific grade(s) that participants attended were identified. In a few instances, it was known
only that students were in high school, as the researchers did not identify the specific grade levels of
the par
ticipants.
Type of student. Participants were labeled as LD (students with a learning disability only), low-achieving
(poor writers only), MH (students with mild handicapping conditions, such as learning disabilities or
speech and language difficulties), Average (average writers; this category did not include the weakest
and strongest writers in a classroom), High (above-average writers only), ESL (students with English as
a second language only), L2 (second language learners only), and Full-Range (representing the full
range of writers found in typical classrooms). It should be noted that LD and MH categories are part
of the broader low-achieving category, but students in these two categories were also receiving special
education services. Each of these groups of youngsters is educated either exclusively or primarily in
the general classroom. Unless indicated otherwise, students with LD and MH were poor writers.
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
40
An additional study feature was coded for investigations in two categories. In the strategy instruction
category, studies were coded as using the Self-Regulated Strategy Development (SRSD) model (Harris
& Graham, 1996, 1999) or not using this model.This was done because Graham (2006) found that the
SRSD model yielded larger effect sizes than did other methods of strategy instruction combined.
Studies in the category entitled “process approach to writing instruction, which involved training of
teachers, were identified as well. Five of the six identified studies involved training through the
National Writing Project (Nagin, 2003). Such training (or the lack of it) may account for variability in
effect sizes for the process writing intervention.
Calculation of Effect Sizes
Effect sizes were calculated as Cohen’s d or the standardized mean difference.The posttest mean
performance of the comparison group was subtracted from that of the intervention group at posttest
and divided by the pooled standard deviation of the two groups.To standardize the calculation of
effect sizes, the learning-to-write effect sizes reported in previous meta-analysis on writing instruction
(Bangert-Drowns, 1993; Goldberg et al., 2003; Graham, 2006; Graham & Harris, 2003; Hillocks, 1986)
were recalculated because a variety of procedures were used in these prior reviews.The writing-to-
learn effect sizes from Bangert-Drowns et al. (2004) were not recalculated, as 23 of the 26 studies
were included in the previous review.
In calculating learning-to-write effect sizes, writing quality was used as the outcome. Holistic quality
scores (a single score that measur
es general overall quality) were used over analytic scales (separate
scor
es for specific aspects of wr
iting, such as content, organization, vocabulary, mechanics, and so
for
th).
If only an analytic scale was available, the scores for mechanics were excluded when calculating
a mean effect size for quality
. For writing-to-learn studies, effect sizes were calculated on the academic
achie
vement measures most similar to typical classroom achievement measures, as was done by
Banger
t-Dr
o
wns et al.
(2004).
A cor
r
ection w
as made for small sample size bias in thr
ee instances in which the sample size w
as less
than 20 (Hedges & Olkin,
1985).
When means or standar
d de
viations w
er
e unr
eported, effect sizes were
calculated fr
om t-tests,
analysis of v
ar
iance (ANO
V
A),
or regression analysis (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
To avoid inflating sample size (Wolf, 1986) and violating the assumption of independence of data,
only one effect size was computed for each study, with two exceptions. One exception involved
comparisons of two interventions in which each intervention fit one of the established groupings
and an effect size was calculated for both categories. For example, Curry (1997) compared strategy
instruction (effect size = 0.51) and the process writing approach (effect size = -0.51).The other
exception involved cases in which more than two different interventions were compared.When this
occurred, an effect size for each intervention was calculated. For example, Knudson (1989) compared
four different interventions.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
41
Statistical Analysis of Effect Sizes
Analyses were conducted only for groupings that included four or more effect sizes based on the
precedents set by Hillocks (1986). Nevertheless, it is recognized that small sample sizes are not very
reliable, and a summary statistic is not reported for groupings with small samples and considerable
variation in effect sizes.
Our meta-analysis employed a weighted fixed-effects model. For each grouping, we calculated the
mean, standard deviation, and median for the unweighted effect sizes.We also calculated the mean and
confidence interval for weighted effect sizes.While it is best to interpret the magnitude of an average
weighted effect size (e.g., strategy instruction in writing) in relation to the distribution of other mean
effect sizes in the same general area (i.e., other writing instructional approaches), in interpreting effect
sizes, we used the generally accepted rule of thumb that an effect size of 0.20 is small, 0.50 is medium,
and 0.80 is large (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
We also conducted a test of homogeneity to determine if the various effect sizes averaged together
in a grouping estimated the same population effect size.When variability in effect sizes was larger
than expected based on sampling error alone (i.e., the homogeneity test was statistically significant),
this excess variability was examined to determine whether it could be accounted for by identifiable
differences between studies (i.e., grade level). Using a fixed-effects model (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001),
the effect size variance was partitioned in order to examine if a specific study feature (a) systematically
differentiated between investigations with larger and smaller effect sizes and (b) accounted for the
excess in variability. MetaWin software (Rosenberg,Adams, & Gurevitch, 2000) was used to conduct
these analyses.
Note that not all of the analyses are included in this document.These can be obtained from Carnegie
Corporation of New York or from a forthcoming article (Graham & Perin, 2006).
Limitations
Readers should keep in mind several caveats and limitations of this report’s findings and
recommendations.
Fir
st, only studies in which the performance of an experimental group was compared with that
of a comparison group were included in this review. Consequently, the conclusions from this
meta-analysis do not reflect the findings from a number of excellent observational studies that
examined the writing practices of effective teachers of writing (e.g., Pressley,Yokoi, Rankin,Wharton-
McDonald, & Mistretta, 1997), studies that measured the correlations between writing performance
and particular teaching procedures (e.g., Applebee, Langer, Nystrand, & Gamoran, 2003), or single-
subject design studies (e.g., De La Paz, 1999). Likewise, it was not possible to draw any conclusions
about the effectiveness of a procedure such as dictation, as most of the research in this area involved
students acting as their own comparison group (e.g., Graham, 1990).
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
42
Second, because the analysis was limited to studies that assessed the quality of students’ writing, we
can draw no conclusions about studies that did not examine effects on writing quality. Consequently,
we were unable to analyze the viability of teaching basic text transcription skills, such as spelling or
handwriting, to adolescents because the search did not yield any studies that examined whether such
instruction had an impact on the writing quality of students in grades 4 through 12.
Third, some instructional procedures have been the focus of more research than others. Although
vocabulary instruction (e.g., Duin & Graves, 1987) may be an effective procedure for improving
students’ writing, for example, not enough research is available to draw even a tentative conclusion
about its impact. In addition, only four areas (strategy instruction, word processing, process writing
approach, and grammar instruction) yielded 10 or more effect sizes. Less confidence can be placed
in the reliability of an average effect size when it is based on a small number of studies.
Four
th, even for some areas that included a large number of effect sizes, not all grade levels were
covered. For strategy instruction, for instance, there were no studies beyond 10th grade.The results of
our meta-analysis do not permit a determination of whether the interventions are effective at specific
grade levels. Interpretation of the findings from this review must be tempered by this fact.
Fifth, in only one instructional approach, strategy instruction, was it possible to examine whether
type of student moderated outcome effects. Even though the authors wanted to determine whether
an instructional approach was effective with low-achieving writers, it was difficult to do so. In addition
the samples of low-achieving writers who participated in the studies in this review represented a mix
of students, including those with and without learning disabilities.
Sixth,
as with Hillocks (1986), a host of subjective decisions had to be made about what constituted
a wr
iting intervention grouping (e.g., process writing approach). In light of the reaction to Hillocks’
r
e
vie
w (see
, e.g., Stotsky, 1988), other researchers will undoubtedly question one or more of the
methodolo
g
ical decisions in the cur
r
ent meta-analysis. As a result, the reasoning process and
decision-making has been made as transpar
ent as possib
le
,
and reliability for the groupings decided
upon is r
epor
ted.
Finally
,
one concer
n with meta-analysis in
v
olv
es ho
w coherent the intervention is for the comparison
condition,
which is also a concer
n in the cur
r
ent r
e
vie
w. For some groupings, such as product goal
setting,
the compar
ison conditions w
er
e homo
geneous.
F
or other groupings, such as the process writing
appr
oach,
ther
e w
as considerab
le v
ar
iation in the comparison conditions. In some of these instances
(e.g., the pr
ocess wr
iting appr
oach),
it was possible to examine whether differences in comparison
conditions w
er
e systematically r
elated to the obtained a
v
erage w
eighted effect size. In other instances,
diversity in compar
ison conditions along with other f
actor
s (e.g., heterogeneity in effect sizes or
variability in the inter
v
entions within a g
r
ouping) resulted in a decision not to report an average
weighted effect size for a g
r
ouping,
and these g
r
oupings are not discussed in the current report.
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
43
APPENDIX B: QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL AND
EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES SUPPORTING
THE 11 KEY ELEMENTS
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
Size
1. Writing Strategies
Curry, K. A. (1997). A comparison of
the writing products of students
with learning disabilities in inclusive
and resource room settings
using different writing instruction
approaches
. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Florida Atlantic
University, Boca Raton, FL.
4 LD Students taught planning
strategies for story writing
versus writing skills
instruction
SRSD
0.51
Glaser, C. (2005). Improving the fourth-
grade students’ composition skills:
Effects of strategy instruction and
self-regulator
y procedures
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Pottsburg, Germany.
4 Full
range
Students taught a planning
strategy for story writing
versus unspecified com-
parison condition
SRSD
1.24
Walser, T. M. (2000). Teaching self-
management of writing strategies
to promote transfer. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Utah State
University, Logan, UT.
4 Full
range
Students taught planning
and revising strategies for
story writing versus direct
instruction in narrative
writing plus journal writing
0.91
Troia, G., & Graham, S. (2002). The
effectiveness of a highly explicit,
teacher-directed strategy instruction
routine: Changing the writing
performance of students with
learning disabilities.
Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 35,
290–305.
4–5 LD Students taught a planning
strategy for persuasive
writing versus partial
process writing model
0.14
MacArthur, C., Schwartz, S., & Graham,
S. (1991). Ef
fects of a r
ecipr
ocal peer
revision strategy in special education
classrooms.
Learning Disability
Research and Practice, 6,
201–210.
4–6 LD Students taught a peer
r
evising strategy for nar
ra
-
tive writing as part of a
process writing approach
versus process writing
instruction only
SRSD
1.09
Details of Quasi-experimental and Experimental Studies Supporting Key Elements
of Adolescent Writing Instruction
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classr
ooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
44
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
A
nderson, A. A. (1997).
T
he effects
of sociocognitive writing strategy
instruction on the writing achieve-
ment and writing self-efficacy of
students with disabilities and typical
achievement in an urban elementary
school
. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Houston,
Houston, TX.
5 F
ull
range
LD
S
tudents taught a planning
strategy for story writing
versus grammar and writ-
ten literature summariza-
tion instruction
SRSD
1
.40
Sawyer, R., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R.
(1992). Direct teaching, strategy
instruction, and strategy instruction
with explicit self-regulation: Effects
on the composition skills and self-
efficacy of students with learning
disabilities.
Jour
nal of Educational
Psychology, 84, 340–352.
5–6 LD Students taught a planning
strategy for story writing
versus writing practice
SRSD
1.86
De La Paz, S., & Graham, S. (1997).
Effects of dictation and advanced
planning instruction on the
composing of students with writing
and learning pr
oblems.
Journal of
Educational Psychology, 89, 203–222.
5–7 LD Students taught a planning
strategy for persuasive
writing versus text
str
ucture instruction on
persuasive essays
SRSD
0.82
Fitzgerald, J., & Markham, L. (1987).
Teaching children about revision in
writing. Cognition and Instruction, 4,
3–24.
6 Full
range
Students taught revising
strategies versus reading
good literature
0.32
Scar
damalia, M., Ber
eiter
, C., &
Steinbach, R. (1984). T
eachability
of reflective processes in written
composition.
Cognitive Science, 8,
173–190.
6 Full
range
Students taught strategies
for being self-r
eflective
when planning versus
unspecified comparison
condition
0.65
Welch, M. (1992). The PLEASE strategy:
A metacognitive learning strategy for
improving the paragraph writing of
students with mild disabilities.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 15,
119–128.
6 LD Students taught a planning
strategy for paragraph
writing versus unspecified
comparison condition
2.26
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
45
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
W
elch, M., & Jensen, J. (1990). Write,
PLEASE: A video-assisted strategic
intervention to improve written
expression of inefficient learners.
Remedial and Special Education, 12,
37–47.
6
–8
Low-
achieving
S
tudents taught a planning
strategy for paragraph
writing versus unspecified
comparison condition
0
.72
Reynolds, C., Hill, D., Swassing, R., &
Ward, M. (1988). The effects of
revision strategy instruction on the
writing performance of students with
learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 21,
540–545.
6–8 LD Students taught editing
and sentence-level
revising strategies versus
directing students to
plan, draft, and revise
compositions
0.16
Gamelin, Y. M. A. (1996). The effects of
Cognitive Strategy Instruction in
Writing (CSIW) on the writing skills
of severely learning disabled stu-
dents and their peers in an inclusive
classroom. Unpublished master’s
thesis, Simon Fraser University,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada.
7 Full
range
Students taught planning
and revising strategies for
compare/contrast essays
versus process writing
instruction
0.98
Yeh, S. (1998). Empowering education:
Teaching argumentative writing to
cultural minority middle-school
students.
Resear
ch in the Teaching
of English, 33, 49–83.
7 Full
range
Students taught a planning
strategy for persuasive
writing versus process
writing appr
oach
0.14
De La Paz, S., & Graham, S. (2002).
Explicitly teaching strategies, skills,
and knowledge: Writing instruction
in middle school classrooms.
Jour
nal of Educational Psychology,
94, 291–304.
7–8 Full
range
Students taught a planning
strategy for persuasive
writing versus traditional
writing instruction
SRSD
0.95
De La Paz, S. (2005). Teaching historical
reasoning and argumentative writing
in culturally and academically
diverse middle school classrooms.
Jour
nal of Educational Psychology
,
97,
139–158.
8 Full
range
Students taught a planning
strategy for writing histori-
cal text versus traditional
writing instruction
SRSD
1.36
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
46
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
S
immons, D. C., Kameenui, E. J.,
Dickson, S., Chard, D., Gunn, B., &
Baker, S. (1994). Integrating narrative
reading comprehension and writing
instruction for all learners. In D. J.
Leu & C. K. Kinzer (Eds.),
Multidimensional aspects of literacy
research, theory, and practice: 43rd
yearbook of The National Reading
Conference
(pp. 572–582). Chicago:
National Reading Conference.
8 F
ull
range
S
tudents taught planning
and revising strategies
for narrative text versus
narrative text structure
instruction and general
writing strategies
0
.40
Wong, B. Y. L., Butler, D. L., Ficzere, S.
A., & Kuperis, S. (1996). Teaching low
achievers and students with learning
disabilities to plan, write, and revise
opinion essays.
Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 29,
197–212.
8–9
Low-
achieving
LD
Students taught planning
and revising strategies
for expository text versus
no-treatment comparison
condition
3.50
Bryson, M., & Scardamalia, M. (1996).
Fostering reflectivity in the argumen-
tative thinking of students with
dif
ferent learning histories.
Reading
and Writing Quar
terly: Overcoming
Learning Difficulties, 12, 351–384.
10 LD
Full
range
Students taught strategies
for reflection when writing
persuasive text versus
instr
uction on basic
elements of persuasive
writing
1.27
2. Summar
ization
Chang, K. E., Sung, Y. T., & Chen, I. D.
(2002). The effect of concept map-
ping to enhance text comprehension
and summarization.
Journal of
Experimental Education, 71, 5–23.
5 Full
range
Students taught to
summarize information by
completing progressively
faded expert summaries of
text versus no-treatment
comparison condition
0.81
Bean, T
. W
., & Steenwyk, F
. L. (1984).
The effect of three forms of summa-
rization instruction on sixth graders’
summar
y writing and compr
ehen-
sion.
Journal of Reading Behavior,
16, 297–306.
6 Full
range
Students taught r
ule-based
strategy to summarize
paragraph-length material
versus advice to write
summaries by finding the
main ideas (i.e., no explicit
instruction)
1.09
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
47
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
K
night, J. E. (2003).
H
elping able
readers synthesize information from
multiple sources in response to
open-ended questions
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
Maryland, College Park, MD.
8 H
igh
S
tudents taught how to
synthesize information
from multiple sources
versus no-treatment
comparison condition
0
.18
Placke, E. (1987). The effect of cognitive
strategy instruction on learning
disabled adolescents’ reading
comprehension and summary
writing. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, State University of
New York, Albany, NY.
10–12 LD Students taught strategies
for summarizing main ideas
in expository material
versus cloze instruction
(i.e., filling in missing
words in reading materials)
1.12
3. Collaborative Writing
Prater, D. L., & Bermúdez, A. B. (1993).
Using peer response groups with
limited English proficient writers.
Bilingual Research Journal, 17,
99–116.
4 ESL Peers helped each other
choose topics as well
as revise and edit text
versus individual work on
compositions
0.19
MacArthur, C., Schwartz, S., & Graham,
S. (1991). Effects of a reciprocal
peer revision strategy in special
education classrooms. Learning
Disability Research and Practice, 6,
201–210.
4–6 LD Peers helped each other
revise and edit text versus
process writing instruction
1.09
Boscolo, P., & Ascorti, K. (2004).
Effects of collaborative revision
on children’s ability to write
understandable narrative texts.
In L. Allal, L. Chanquoy, & P. Largy
(Eds.),
Revision: Cognitive and
instructional processes (pp.
157–170). Boston: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
4, 6, 8 Full
range
Peers helped each other
revise text versus teacher
correction of text
0.96
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
48
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
Y
arrow, F., & Topping, K. J. (2001).
Collaborative writing: The effects
of metacognitive prompting and
structured peer interaction.
British
Journal of Educational Psychology,
71, 261–282.
5
–6
F
ull
range
P
eers helped each other
draft, revise, and edit text
after receiving training in
a process for composing
text versus individual
composition after
training in a process
for composing text
0
.58
Olson, V. B. (1990). The revising
processes of sixth-grade writers
with and without peer feedback.
Journal of Educational Research,
84, 22–29.
6 Full
range
Peers provided each other
with feedback on their
compositions versus
grammar instruction and
individual writing using
process writing approach
0.42
Hill, B. G. (1990). A comparison of
the writing quality of paired and
unpaired students composing at
the computer. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Texas,
Austin.
8 High Peers composed together
on a word processor
versus individual writing
on a word processor
0.46
Dailey, E. M. (1991). The relative
efficacy of cooperative learning
versus individualized learning
on the written performance of
adolescent students with writing
problems. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, John Hopkins
University, Baltimore, MD.
9–12 LD
Low-
achieving
Peers helped each other
plan, draft, and revise
text versus individuals
composing alone
1.18
4. Specific Product Goals
Graham, S., MacArthur, C., &
Schwar
tz, S. (1995). Ef
fects of goal
setting and procedural facilitation
on the r
evising behavior and writing
per
formance of students with
writing and learning problems.
Jour
nal of Educational Psychology
,
87,
230–240.
4–6 LD Goal to add three pieces
of infor
mation while r
evis
-
ing versus goal to make
paper better
0.77
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
49
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
S
chunk, D. H., & Swartz, C. W. (1993).
Goals and progress feedback:
Effects on self-efficacy and writing
achievement.
Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 18,
337–354.
5 F
ull
range
G
oal to write a certain
type of paragraph versus
goal to do best
1
.69
Schunk, D. H., & Swartz, C. W. (1993b).
Writing strategy instruction with
gifted students: Effects of goals and
feedback on self-efficacy and skills.
Roeper Review, 15, 225–230.
5 Full
range
Goal to write a certain
type of paragraph versus
goal to do best
1.01
Ferretti, R. P., MacArthur, C. A., &
Dowdy, N. S. (2000). The effects
of an elaborated goal on the
persuasive writing of students
with learning disabilities and their
nor
mally achieving peers.
Jour
nal
of Educational Psychology, 92,
694–702.
6, 8 Full
range
Goal to include common
persuasive elements ver-
sus goal to write persua-
sive paper
0.38
Page-Voth, V., & Graham, S. (1999).
Effects of goal setting and strategy
use on the writing performance and
self-efficacy of students with writ-
ing and lear
ning problems. Jour
nal
of Educational Psychology, 91,
230–240.
7–8 LD Goal to include common
persuasive elements
versus goal to write
persuasive paper
1.18
5. Word Processing
Cheever, M. S. (1987). The effects
of using a word processor on
the acquisition of composition
skills by the elementary student.
Unpublished doctoral disser
tation,
Northwestern University,
Evanston, IL.
4 Full
range
Students composed on
computer 1 day a week
0.30
Jackiewicz, G. (1995). The effect of
computer based instruction on
writing at the elementar
y level
.
(ERIC Document Reproduction
Ser
vice No. ED380802)
4 Full
range
Students used word
processor during
computer lab for 12 weeks
1.74
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
50
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
M
oore, M. A. (1987).
T
he effect of word
processing technology in a develop-
mental writing program on writing
quality, attitude towards composing,
and revision strategies of fourth and
fifth grade students
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
South Florida, Tampa, FL.
4
–5
F
ull
range
S
tudents used word
processor as part of
writing program for
more than 10 weeks
0
.44
Lichtenstein, N. (1996). The effect
of word processing on writing
achievement
. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED394146)
5 Full
range
Students used word
processor for all writing
assignments for 20 weeks
0.75
Espinoza, S. F. (1992). The effects of
using a word processor containing
grammar and spell checkers on the
composition writing of sixth graders.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX.
6 Full
range
Students composed
with word processor for
6 weeks
0.03
Miller, S. K. (1984). Plugging your pencil
into the wall: An investigation of
word processing and writing skills at
the middle school level. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
Oregon, Eugene, OR.
6 High Students composed
paragraphs on word
processor over 4 weeks
-0.09
Hagler
, W. J. (1993).
The ef
fects of the
word processor on the revision
behaviors of sixth grade students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Aubur
n University, Auburn, AL.
6 Full
range
Students wr
ote and
revised compositions on
word processor for 1 week
0.97
Dybdahl, C. S., Shaw, D. G., & Blahous,
E. (1997). The impact of the computer
on writing: No simple answers.
Computers in the Schools, 13, 41–53.
6 Full
range
Students used
word processor for
all assignments for
28 weeks
-0.18
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
51
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
L
owther, D. L., Ross, S. M., & Morrison,
G. M. (2003). When each one has
one: The influences on teaching
strategies and student achievement
of using laptops in the classroom.
Educational Technology, Research
and Development, 51, 23–44.
6
–7
F
ull
range
S
tudents had round-the-
clock access to laptop
computers for academic
year
1
.11
Shinn, J. A. (1986). The effectiveness
of word processing and problem
solving computer use on the skills
of learning disabled students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
United States International
University, San Diego, CA.
6–8 LD
Full
range
Students composed with
word processor and
received problem-solving
instruction on computer for
12 weeks
1.38
Lytle, M. J. (1987). Word processors
and writing: The relation of seventh
grade students’ learner characteris-
tics and revision behaviors.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.
7 Full
range
Students used word
processor to write and
revise all assignments
-0.05
Dalton, D. W., & Hannafin, M. J. (1987).
The effects of word processing on
written composition.
Journal of
Educational Resear
ch, 80,
338–342.
7
Low-
achieving
Students used word
processor for writing
assignments for academic
year
0.28
Ler
ew, E. L. (1997).
The use of comput
-
ers to improve writing skills among
low-achieving Hispanic students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of La V
erne, La Verne, CA.
8
Low-
achieving
Students used wor
d
processor for writing
assignments for 20 weeks
0.89
Head, B. B. (2000). Revision instruction
and quality of writing by eighth
grade students using paper and
pencil or word processing.
Unpublished doctoral disser
tation,
Oakland University, Rochester, MI.
8 Full
range
Students used word
processor for 4 weeks
0.01
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
52
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
Lam, F. S., & Pennington, M. C. (1995).
The computer vs. the pen: A
comparative study of word process-
ing in a Hong Kong secondary
classroom. Computer Assisted
Language Learning, 8, 75–92.
9 Low-
achieving
Students composed
with word processor for
academic year
0.33
Philhower, S. C. (1985). The effects of
the use of a word processing pro-
gram on the writing skills of mildly
handicapped secondary students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
9–12 MH Students composed with
word processor for 16
weeks
0.51
Cirello, V. J. (1986). The effect of word
processing on the writing abilities
of tenth grade remedial writing
students. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, New York University,
New York.
10 Low-
achieving
Students received 20
weeks of remedial writing
instruction while using
word processor
1.10
Silver, N. W., & Repa, J. T. (1993).
The effect of word processing
on the quality of writing and self-
esteem of secondary school
English-as-second-language
students: Writing without censure.
Journal of Educational Computing
Research, 9,
265–283.
9–12 ESL Students composed with
word processor for 13
weeks
0.52
6.
Sentence-Combining
Saddler, B., & Graham, S. (2005). The
effects of peer-assisted sentence-
combining instruction on the writing
performance of more and less
skilled young writers.
Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97, 43–54.
4 Average
Low-
achieving
Sentence-combining
instruction versus
grammar instruction
0.42
Stoddard, E. P. (1982). The combined
effect of creative thinking and
sentence-combining activities on
the writing ability of above average
ability fifth and sixth grade students.
Unpublished doctoral disser
tation,
University of Connecticut, Storrs.
5–6 High Sentence-combining
instruction versus
unspecified comparison
condition
0.66
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
53
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
Pedersen, E. L. (1977). Improving
syntactic and semantic fluency in
writing of language arts students
through extended practice in
sentence-combining. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
Minnesota, Minneapolis.
7 Full
range
Sentence-combining
instruction added to tradi-
tional literacy instruction
versus traditional literacy
instruction
0.40
Howie, S. M. H. (1979). A study: The
effects of sentence combining
practice on the writing ability
and reading level of ninth grade
students
. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Colorado,
Boulder.
9 Full
range
Sentence-combining
instruction versus
grammar instruction
0.21
Kanellas, R., Carifio, J., & Dagostino, L.
(1998). Improving the expository
writing skills of adolescents. Oxford
University Press, New York.
9 Average Sentence-combining
instruction versus
grammar instruction
0.61
7. Prewriting
Loader, L. M. (1989). The effects of
the semantic organizer on writing
ability and motivation with fourth
grade students. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
Connecticut, Storrs.
4 Full
range
Students completed a
semantic web versus
listing ideas for writing
0.32
Brodney, B., Reeves, C., & Kazelskis, R.
(1999). Selected prewriting
treatments: Effects on expository
compositions written by fifth-grade
students. Journal of Experimental
Education, 68, 5–20.
5 Full
range
Students read information
on topic and were prompt-
ed to plan versus listening
to infor
mation on topic
0.95
Br
odney
, B., Reeves, C., & Kazelskis, R.
(1999). Selected prewriting
treatments: Effects on expository
compositions written by fifth-grade
students. Journal of Experimental
Education, 68, 5–20.
5 Full
range
Students pr
ompted to
plan a paper after listening
to information on topic
versus only listening to
information on topic
0.17
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
54
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
R
eece, J. E., & Cumming, G. (1996).
Evaluating speech-based composi-
tion methods: Planning, dictation,
and the listening word processor.
In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.),
The science of writing: Theories,
methods, individual differences,
and applications (pp. 361–380).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (Study 4)
5
–6
F
ull
range
S
tudents encouraged
to plan after brief
demonstration of how
to do so versus no-
treatment comparison
condition
0
.61
Vinson, L. L. N. (1980). The effects of
two prewriting activities upon the
overall quality of ninth graders’
descriptive paragraphs. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
South Carolina, Columbia.
9 Full
range
Groups of students
brainstorm ideas for paper,
discuss which ideas to
include, and organize
their ideas prior to writing
versus writing paragraphs
with emphasis on
correction of first draft
0.06
8. Inquiry Activities
Hillocks, G., Jr. (1982). The interaction of
instruction, teacher comment, and
revision in teaching the composing
process. Research in the Teaching
of English, 16,
261–278.
7–8 Full
range
Students examined or
observed various activities
and collected data to write
about them versus writing
that was facilitated by
teacher discussion
0.14
Hillocks, G., Jr. (1982). The interaction of
instr
uction, teacher comment, and
revision in teaching the composing
process.
Research in the Teaching
of English, 16,
261–278.
7–8 Full
range
Students examined or
obser
ved various activities
and collected data to write
about them and then
revised papers versus
writing that was facilitated
by student discussion
-0.05
Hillocks, G., Jr. (1979). The effects of
observational activities on student
writing. Research in the Teaching of
English, 13, 23–35.
9, 11 Full
range
Students examined or
observed various activities
and collected data to
write about them
versus instr
uction in
paragraph writing
0.75
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
55
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
W
idvey, L. I. H. (1971).
A
study of the
use of a problem-solving approach
to composition in high school
English
. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Nebraska,
Lincoln.
1
1
F
ull
range
S
tudents formulated
hypotheses, gathered and
analyzed data, and made
inferences to structure
writing versus traditional
writing instruction
0
.65
Pisano, R. C. (1980). The effectiveness
of an intervention study in critical
thinking skills designed to improve
written composition in eleventh
and twelfth graders. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Rutgers:
The State University of New Jersey,
New Brunswick.
11–12 Full
range
Students asked to respond
to questions designed
to engage them in
critical thinking about
five literature topics versus
teachers’ regular question-
ing techniques
-0.07
9. Process Writing Approach
Umbach, B. T. (1990). A comparison
of two methods of teaching written
language to low-performing fourth
graders in two rural schools.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Auburn University, Auburn, AL.
4
Low-
achieving
Process writing approach
versus instruction in
strategies for drafting a
paper
-0.03
Curry, K. A. (1997). A comparison of
the writing products of students
with learning disabilities in inclusive
and resour
ce room settings using
dif
fer
ent writing instr
uction
appr
oaches
. Unpublished doctoral
disser
tation, Florida Atlantic
University
, Boca Raton, FL.
4 LD Process writing approach
versus skills instruction
0.69
T
r
oia, G., & Graham, S. (2002). The
effectiveness of a highly explicit,
teacher-directed strategy instruction
r
outine: Changing the writing
performance of students with
lear
ning disabilities.
Jour
nal of
Learning Disabilities, 35,
290–305.
4–5 LD Modified pr
ocess writing
approach versus strategy
instruction
-0.14
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
56
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
G
orka, D. M. (1992).
T
he effects of a
staff development program in writing
process on learners’ writing skills
and attitudes toward writing
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Pennsylvania State University,
University Park.
4
–6
F
ull
range
S
taff training in process
writing approach versus
unspecified comparison
condition with no training
TRAINING STUDY
0
.83
Pantier, T. F. (1999). A comparison of
writing performance of fifth grade
students using the process writing
approach and the Shurley Method
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Oklahoma State University,
Stillwater.
5 Full
range
Process writing
approach versus
grammar instruction
-0.30
Moye, M. J. (1993). The impact of a
cognitive strategy on students’
composing skills. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, College of
William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA.
5 Full
range
Process writing approach
(including models and
scales) versus teaching
students to use graphic
organizers
0.48
Robinson, M. E. (1986). The writing
performance and revision behavior
of fifth grade process and non-
pr
ocess writing students during one-
day and two-day writing sessions.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Maine, Orono.
5 Full
range
Process writing
approach versus
traditional instruction
0.28
Varble, M. E. (1990). Analysis of writing
samples of students taught by teach
-
ers using whole language and tradi-
tional approaches.
Journal of
Educational Resear
ch, 83,
245–251.
6 Full
range
Whole-language
instr
uction with pr
ocess
writing instruction versus
skills instruction
-0.11
Gamelin, Y. M. A. (1996). The effects
of Cognitive Strategy Instr
uction in
W
riting (CSIW) on the writing skills
of severely learning disabled stu-
dents and their peers in an inclusive
classroom. Unpublished master’s
thesis, Simon Fraser University
,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada.
7 Full
range
Process writing approach
versus strategy instr
uction
-0.98
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
57
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
H
ayes, B. L. (1984).
T
he effects of
implementing process writing into
a seventh grade English curriculum.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Delta State University, Cleveland,
MS.
7 F
ull
range
P
rocess writing approach
versus traditional grammar
instruction
0
.22
Yeh, S. (1998). Empowering education:
Teaching argumentative writing to
cultural minority middle-school stu-
dents. Research in the Teaching of
English, 33, 49–83.
7 Full
range
Process writing approach
versus strategy instruction
-0.14
Olson, M. C., & DiStefano, P. (1980).
Describing and testing the
effectiveness of a contemporary
model for in-service education in
teaching composition.
Engineering
Education, 12, 69–76.
7-9 Full
range
National Writing Project
training in process
writing approach versus
unspecified comparison
condition with no training
TRAINING STUDY
0.40
Ganong, F. L. (1974). Teaching writing
through the use of a program based
on the work of Donald M. Murray.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Boston University School of
Education, Boston, MA.
9 Average,
high
Early form of process
writing approach versus
more traditional approach
in which students follow
prescribed series of
writing exercises
-0.13
Roberts, C. (2002). The influence of
teachers’ professional development
at the Tampa Bay Area Writing
Project on student writing
performance
. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation: University of South
Florida, Tampa, FL.
6–12 Full
range
National Writing Project
training in process writing
approach versus
unspecified comparison
condition with no training
TRAINING STUDY
0.14
Pritchard, R. J. (1987). Effects on
student writing of teacher training in
the National Writing Project Model.
Written Communication, 4, 51–67.
7–12 Full
range
National Writing Project
training in process writing
approach versus unspeci-
fied comparison condition
with minimal or no training
TRAINING STUDY
0.38
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
58
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
P
ritchard, R. J., & Marshall, J. C. (1994).
Evaluation of a tiered model for staff
development in writing. Research in
the Teaching of English, 28,
259–285.
7
–12
F
ull
range
N
ational Writing Project
training in process writing
approach versus
unspecified comparison
condition with no training
TRAINING STUDY
0
.50
Alloway, E., Carroll, J., Emig, J., King, B.,
Marcotrigiano, I., Smith, J., & Spicer,
W. (1979). The New Jersey Writing
Project
. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University, Educational Testing
Program Service, and Nineteen New
Jersey Public School Districts.
7–12 Full
range
National Writing Project
training in process writing
approach versus
unspecified comparison
condition with no training
TRAINING STUDY
0.39
Gauntlett, J. F. (1978). Project WRITE
and its effect on the writing of high
school students. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Northern
Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ.
10–12 Full
range
Process writing
approach versus
traditional instruction
0.02
Adams, V. A. (1971). A study of the
effects of two methods of teaching
composition to twelfth graders.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Illinois, Champaign-
Urbana.
12 High Early form of process writ-
ing approach versus skills
instr
uction
0.28
Reimer, M. (2001). The effect of a
traditional, a process writing,
and a combined talking and writing
instr
uctional approach on the quality
of secondar
y English students’
written response. Unpublished
master’
s thesis, University of
Manitoba, Canada.
9–12 Full
range
Process writing
approach versus
traditional instruction
-1.00
Scannella, A. M. (1982). A writing-as-
process model as a means for
improving compositions and
attitudes toward composition in
the high school. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Rutgers:
The State University of New Jersey,
New Br
unswick.
9–12 Full
range
Process writing
approach versus
traditional instruction
0.14
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
59
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
10. Study of Models
Knudson, R. E. (1989). Effects of
instructional strategies on children’s
informational writing. Journal of
Educational Research, 83,
91–96.
4, 6, 8 High Students examined model
pieces of writing to direct
composition of their
papers versus free writing
0.26
Knudson, R. E. (1991). Effects of
instructional strategies, grade,
and sex on students’ persuasive
writing. Journal of Experimental
Education, 59, 141–152.
4, 6, 8 Full
range
Students examined model
pieces of writing to direct
composition of their
papers versus free writing
0.24
Thibodeau, A. E. (1964). Improving
composition writing with grammar
and organization exercises utilizing
differentiated group patterns.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Boston University School of
Education, Boston, MA.
6 Full
range
Students examined model
pieces of writing for both
narrative and expository
writing versus traditional
language arts instruction
0.44
Reedy, J. E., Jr. (1964). A comparative
study of two methods of teaching
the organization of expository
writing to ninth-grade pupils.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Boston University School of
Education, Boston, MA.
9 Full
range
Students examined
model composition for
six patterns used to
organize expository
writing versus instruction
in the process of
communication in writing
0.26
Vinson, L. L. N. (1980). The effects
of two prewriting activities upon
the overall quality of ninth graders’
descriptive paragraphs.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of South Carolina,
Columbia.
9 Full
range
Students examined models
used to illustrate concrete
detail, sensory imagery,
unnecessary detail, and
single impression versus
writing paragraphs with
emphasis on correction of
first drafts
-0.29
Caplan, R., & Keech, C. (1980). Show-
writing: A training program to help
students be specific
. (Collaborative
Resear
ch Study No. 2). Berkeley
,
CA: Bay Area Writing Project. (ERIC
Document Repr
oduction Ser
vice
No. ED198539)
12 Full
range
Students examined
models used to illustrate
difference between
showing and telling
0.11
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
60
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
11. Writing for Content Learning
Davis, B. H. (1990). The effects of
expressive writing on the social
studies achievement, writing fluency,
and learning retention of fourth-
grade students
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech
University, Lubbock.
4 Full
range
Social studies: Students
made journal entries about
social studies materials
0.12
Millican, B. R. (1994). The effects of
writing-to-learn tasks on achieve-
ment and attitude in mathematics
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of North Texas, Denton.
4 Full
range
Math: Students used
writing activities to facili-
tate math learning
0.59
Lodholz, R. D. (1980). The effects of
student composition of
mathematical verbal problems
on student problem solving
performance. Unpublished doctoral
disser
tation, University of Missouri,
Columbia.
4–5 Full
range
Math: Students wrote
verbal descriptions of math
problems
-0.02
Boscolo, P., & Mason, L. (2001). W
riting
to learn, writing to transfer. In G.
Rijlaarsdam, P. Tynjala, L. Mason, &
K. Lonka (Eds.),
Studies in writing:
Vol. 7. W
riting as a learning tool:
Integrating theory and practice
(pp. 83–104). The Netherlands:
Kluwer Academic Publishers.
5 Full
range
History: Students used
writing for note-taking,
commenting, synthesizing,
reflection, and expressing
doubt
1.04
Bauman, M. A. (1992). The effect of
teacher-directed journal writing on
fifth-grade student mathematics
achievement. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Marquette University,
Milwaukee, WI.
5 Full
range
Math: Students used
writing-to-learn activities
during math instruction
0.23
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
61
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
M
adden, B. R. (1992).
A
n investigation
of the relationship between
journal writing and mathematics
achievement in fifth grade students
in a rural unit school district
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Southern Illinois University,
Edwardsville.
5 F
ull
range
M
ath: Students completed
journal entries in response
to prompts about math
material
0
.33
Moynihan, C. M. (1994). A model and
study of the role of communication
in the mathematics learning process
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Boston College, Boston, MA.
5 Full
range
Math: Students wrote and
shared journal entries
about math activities
0.86
Dipillo, M. L. (1994). A quantitative/
qualitative analysis of student
journal writing in middle-grade
mathematics classes. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of
Akron, Akron, OH.
5–6 Full
range
Math: Students made
journal entries as part of
math instruction
0.52
Baisch, C. L. (1990). Writing methods
used in the teaching of mathematics:
An empirical study. Unpublished
master’s thesis, Eastern Michigan
University, Ypsilanti.
6 Full
range
Math: Students used
writing methods developed
in English to learn math
-0.21
Konopak, B. C., Mar
tin, S. H., & Martin,
M. A. (1990). Using a writing strategy
to enhance sixth-grade students’
comprehension of content material.
Jour
nal of Reading Behavior, 22,
19–36.
6 Full
range
Histor
y: Students used
writing to explore historical
knowledge
-0.01
Shepard, R. G. (1992). Using writing
for conceptual development in
mathematics instruction.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Indiana University
, Bloomington.
6 Full
range
Math: Students completed
writing assignments for
math homework
0.20
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
62
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
A
yers, W. E. (1993).
A
study of the
effectiveness of expressive writing
as a learning enhancement in
middle school science
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Temple
University, Philadelphia, PA.
6
–8
F
ull
range
E
arth science: Students
used expressive writing
activities to explore earth
science
-
0.77
Rivard, L. P. (1996). The effect of talking
and writing, alone and combined,
on learning in science: An
exploratory study. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University
of Manitoba, Canada.
8 Full
range
Science: Students wrote
about tasks involving
scientific explanation
for real-world ecology
problems
-0.46
Willey, L. H. (1988). The effects of
selected writing-to-learn approach-
es on high school students’ attitudes
and achievement. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Mississippi
State University, Mississippi State.
8 Full
range
Social studies: Students
wrote journal entries and
wrote about social studies
material
0.04
Reaves, R. R. (1991). The effects of
writing-to-learn activities on the
content knowledge, retention of
information, and attitudes toward
writing of selected vocational
agriculture education students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
North Carolina State University,
Raleigh.
9 Full
range
Natural resources:
Students completed
various writing activities
to promote learning about
protecting ground water
-0.12
Johnson, L. A. (1991). Ef
fects of essay
writing on achievement in algebra
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Connecticut, Stor
rs.
9 Full
range
Algebra: Students pr
ovided
instr
uction in how to
answer math essay
questions
0.55
Stewart, C. B. (1992). Journal writing
in mathematics classrooms: A
practical inquiry. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation, Memphis State
University
, Memphis, TN.
9–10 Full
range
Algebra: Students wrote in
journals about class math
activities
0.59
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
63
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
L
anger, J. A., & Applebee, A. N. (1987).
How writing shapes thinking.
Urbana, IL: National Council of
Teachers of English. (Study 2)
9
, 11
F
ull
range
S
ocial studies: Students
used writing to reformulate
and extend social studies
material
-
0.13
Langer, J. A., & Applebee, A. N. (1987).
How writing shapes thinking.
Urbana, IL: National Council of
Teachers of English. (Study 3)
9, 11 Full
range
Social studies: Students
wrote summaries to review
new material
0.18
Willey, L. H. (1988). The effects of
selected writing-to-lear
n
appr
oaches on high school
students’ attitudes and achievement
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Mississippi State University,
Mississippi State.
10 Full
range
Biology: Students wrote
jour
nal entries and wrote
about biology activities
1.48
Hand, B., Hohenshell, L., & Prain, V.
(2004). Exploring students’ responses
to conceptual questions when
engaged with planned writing
experiences: A study with year
ten science students. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 41,
186–210.
10 Full
range
Biology: Students
completed two writing
assignments versus one
writing assignment
0.77
Kasparek, R. F. (1993). Effects of
integrated writing on attitude
and algebra performance of high
school students
. Unpublished
doctoral disser
tation, University
of North Carolina, Greensboro.
11 Full
range
Algebra: Students
completed two writing
assignments versus one
writing assignment
0.37
Wong, B. Y. L., Kuperis, S., Jamieson, D.,
Keller, L., & Cull-Hewitt, R. (2002).
Effects of guided journal writing on
students’ story understanding.
Jour
nal of Educational Research, 95
,
179–191.
12 Full
range
English: Students made
journal entries structured
with general response
questions about material
r
ead
1.68
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
64
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
B
ell, E. S., & Bell, R. N. (1985). Writing
and mathematical problem solving:
Arguments in favor of synthesis.
School Science and Mathematics,
85, 210–221.
9
–12
F
ull
range
M
ath: Students solved
math problems using
writing to record steps
0
.27
Licata, K. P. (1993). Writing about
mathematical relations in science:
Effects of achievement
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, State
University of New York, Buffalo.
9–12 Full
range
Math in science: Students
wrote about mathematical
relationships in science
0.27
Nieswandt, M. (1997, March).
Improving learning in chemistry
classes through original writing
about chemical facts
. Paper
presented at the annual meeting
of the American Educational
Research Association, Chicago.
9–12 Full
range
Chemistry: Students wrote
about chemical facts
0.12
Traditional Grammar Instruction
Saddler, B., & Graham, S. (2005).
The effects of peer-assisted
sentence-combining instruction
on the writing performance of
more and less skilled young writers.
Journal of Educational Psychology,
97, 43–54.
4 Average
Low-
achieving
Traditional grammar
instruction versus
sentence-combining
-0.42
Anderson, A. A. (1997). The effects
of sociocognitive writing
strategy instruction on the
writing achievement and writing
self-efficacy of students with
disabilities and typical achievement
in an urban elementary school
.
Unpublished doctoral disser
tation,
University of Houston, Houston, TX.
5 Full
range
LD
Traditional grammar
instruction versus planning
or revising strategy
instruction
-1.40
Writing Next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools
65
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
Average = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
approach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
Pantier, T. F. (1999). A comparison of
writing performance of fifth grade
students using the process writing
approach and the Shurley Method
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Oklahoma State University,
Stillwater.
5 Full
range
Traditional grammar
instruction versus process
writing approach
0.30
Thibodeau, A. L. (1964). A study of the
effects of elaborative thinking and
vocabulary enrichment exercises on
written composition
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Boston
University School of Education,
Boston, MA.
6 Full
range
Traditional grammar
instruction versus peers
working on elaborative
thinking and vocabulary
enrichment activities
-0.54
Thibodeau, A. L. (1964). A study of the
effects of elaborative thinking and
vocabular
y enrichment exercises on
written composition. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Boston
University School of Education,
Boston, MA.
6 Full
range
Traditional grammar
instruction versus
individual students
working on self-directing,
elaborative thinking, and
vocabulary enrichment
activities
-0.41
Howie, S. M. H. (1979). A study: The
effects of sentence combining
practice on the writing ability and
reading level of ninth grade students
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Colorado, Boulder.
9 Full
range
Traditional grammar
instruction versus
sentence-combining
-0.21
Hayes, B. L. (1984). The ef
fects of
implementing process writing into
a seventh grade English cur
riculum
.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Delta State University, Cleveland,
MS.
7 Full
range
T
raditional grammar
instruction versus process
writing appr
oach
-0.22
A Report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
66
LD = students with learning disability
MH = students with mild handicapping conditions
ESL = students with English as second language
Low-achieving = low-achieving writers who were not LD or MH
A
verage = average writers
High = above average writers
Full range = full range of writers found in typical classrooms
SRSD = Self-Regulated Strategy Development study
TRAINING STUDY = professional development in process writing
appr
oach
Study Grades Students Instructional Approach Effect
S
ize
Kanellas, R., Carifio, J., & Dagostino, L.
(1998).
Improving the expository
writing skills of adolescents
. Oxford
University Press, New York.
9 Average Traditional grammar
instruction versus
sentence-combining
-0.61
Fearn, L., & Farnan, N. (2005, April).
An investigation of the influence
of teaching grammar in writing
to accomplish an influence on
writing
. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the American
Educational Research Association,
Montreal, Canada.
10 Full
range
Traditional grammar
instruction versus gram-
mar instruction in context
1.07
Elley, W. B., Barham, I. H., Lamb, H., &
Wyllie, M. (1975). The role of
grammar in a secondary school
English curriculum.
New Zealand
Journal of Educational Studies, 10,
26–42.
11 Average Transformational grammar
instruction versus reading
and writing
0.00
Elley, W. B., Barham, I. H., Lamb, H., &
Wyllie, M. (1975). The r
ole of
grammar in a secondary school
English curriculum.
New Zealand
Journal of Educational Studies, 10,
26–42.
11 Average Traditional grammar
instruction versus r
eading
and writing
0.03
Want to receive the latest information on high school policy?
Add your name to the Alliance’s mailing list!
The Alliance for Excellent Education promotes
high school transformation to make it possible for every
child to graduate prepared for postsecondary education
and success in life.
A
Washington-based policy, research, and advocacy
organization,
the Alliance focuses on issues of concern
to middle and high schools, including adolescent literacy
,
school leadership
, college preparation, dropout reduction,
and the use of data to inform decisionmaking.
To add your name to the Alliance mailing list, visit
http://www.all4ed.org/whats_at_stake/mailinglist.html or fill
out the following form and mail it to the Alliance for Excellent
Education at 1201 Connecticut Avenue N.W., Suite 901,
Washington, DC 20036. You may also fax the form to
202-828-0821. If you have questions, call 202-828-0828.
Name__________________________________________
Title___________________________________________
O
rganization
____________________________________
Address_________________________________________
City/S
tate/Z
ip ___________________________________
Phone ___________________Fax ___________________
E
mail addr
es
s
____________________________________
(E
mail addr
ess is r
equir
ed to r
eceive
S
tr
aight A
s
.)
Want to receive the latest information on high school policy?
Add your name to the Alliance’s mailing list!
The Alliance for Excellent Education promotes
high school transformation to make it possible for every
child to graduate prepared for postsecondary education
and success in life.
A Washington-based policy, research, and advocacy
organization, the Alliance focuses on issues of concern
to middle and high schools, including adolescent literacy,
school leadership, college preparation, dropout reduction,
and the use of data to inform decisionmaking.
To add your name to the Alliance mailing list, visit
http://www.all4ed.org/whats_at_stake/mailinglist.html or fill
out the following form and mail it to the Alliance for Excellent
Education at 1201 Connecticut Avenue N.W., Suite 901,
Washington, DC 20036. You may also fax the form to
202-828-0821. If you have questions, call 202-828-0828.
Name__________________________________________
Title___________________________________________
Organization ____________________________________
Address_________________________________________
City/State/Zip ___________________________________
Phone ___________________Fax ___________________
Email address____________________________________
(Email address is required to receive Straight A’s.)
Alliance for Excellent Education
1201 Connecticut Avenue, NW
Suite 901
Washington, DC 20036-2605
Place
Postage
Here
Straight A’s focuses on education news and events in
Washington, DC, and around the country. The format
makes information on national education issues accessible
to everyone from elected officials and policymakers to
parents and community leaders. Learn about emerging
research, promising practices, and policy decisions that are
helping to shape secondary school reform in America.
The Alliance publishes cutting-edge reports such as
Reading Next that combine the best research currently
available with well-crafted strategies for turning that
research into practice.
Alliance for Excellent Education
1201 Connecticut Avenue, NW
S
uite 901
Washington, DC 20036-2605
P
lace
Postage
H
ere
Straight A’s focuses on education news and events in
Washington, DC, and around the country. The format
makes information on national education issues accessible
to everyone from elected officials and policymakers to
parents and community leaders. Learn about emerging
research, promising practices, and policy decisions that are
helping to shape secondar
y school reform in America.
The Alliance publishes cutting-edge r
eports such as
Reading Next that combine the best research currently
available with well-crafted strategies for turning that
research into practice.
www.all4ed.org