Georgia State University Georgia State University
ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University
Communication Honors Theses Department of Communication
Spring 5-7-2016
Labeling Terrorism: Media Effects on Public Policy Attitudes Labeling Terrorism: Media Effects on Public Policy Attitudes
Valentina Garzon
Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/communication_hontheses
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Garzon, Valentina, "Labeling Terrorism: Media Effects on Public Policy Attitudes." Thesis, Georgia State
University, 2016.
doi: https://doi.org/10.57709/8743123
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Communication at ScholarWorks @
Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Communication Honors Theses by an authorized
administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact
LABELING TERRORISM: MEDIA EFFECTS ON PUBLIC POLICY ATTITUDES
by
VALENTINA GARZON
Under the direction of Anthony Lemieux, Ph.D.
ABSTRACT
When is an attack perceived as terrorism? Anecdotally, violent events in the media are
more often labeled terrorism when the perpetrator is Muslim. The media has profound effects on
the public’s perception of issues such as terrorism. However, public views on labeling terrorism
have not been tested empirically. This research aims to explore how the public perceives a
violent event depending on the context. Using a 2 (Muslim vs. Christian perpetrator) x2 (male vs.
female perpetrator) x2 (unknown vs. political motive) experimental design, participants were
presented with varying conditions of an attack. They were asked to report their perceptions of the
attack and perpetrator, as well as their attitudes towards different policies. Findings show that
attacks with Muslim perpetrators were more likely to be labeled terrorism, and the terrorism
label is correlated with support for policies negatively affecting Muslims.
INDEX WORDS: Terrorism, Media, Violence, Muslims, Public attitudes, Public policy
LABELING TERRORISM: MEDIA EFFECTS ON PUBLIC POLICY ATTITUDES
by
VALENTINA GARZON
An Honors Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for
Undergraduate Research Honors
in the Honors College
Georgia State University
2016
Copyright by
Valentina Garzon
2016
LABELING TERRORISM: MEDIA EFFECTS ON PUBLIC POLICY ATTITUDES
by
VALENTINA GARZON
Honors Thesis Advisor: Anthony Lemieux
Honors College Associate Dean: Sarah Cook
Electronic Version Approved:
Honors College
Georgia State University
2016
v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I would like to take the time to thank my advisor, Dr. Lemieux, for guiding me
throughout this project. By his encouragement, I applied to the National Consortium for the
Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism’s Undergraduate Research Program (URP),
which provides research opportunities for undergraduates in the behavioral sciences in the area
of terrorism and homeland security. I was selected as one of the five finalists for the program and
was given partial funding for my research study. In addition, the time he took to help me to
create my study, obtain IRB approval, and give me feedback has been invaluable in the success
of this paper.
I would also like to thank my family and friends, as they have provided much support to
me during my college career, and especially, during the time that I completed my honors thesis.
There were times when I doubted myself, but they were there to lift me up and encourage me to
continue.
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ………………………………………………………………………v
LIST OF TABLES …………………………………………………………………………..viii
LIST OF FIGURES ……………………………………………………………………………...ix
INTRODUCTION ………………………………………………………………………………..1
Terrorism in the Media …………………………………………………………………...2
Muslims in the Media …………………………………………………………………….2
Female Terrorists in the Media …………………………………………………………...3
Media’s Effect on American Public Policy Attitudes …………………………………….4
Which Factors Affect Terrorism Labeling? ………………………………………………7
METHOD ………………………………………………………………………………………...8
Amazon Mechanical Turk ………………………………………………………………..8
Survey Measures …………………………………………………………………………9
Attack Labeling ………………………………………………………………….9
General Attitudes ………………………………………………………………..10
Policy Attitudes ………………………………………………………………….11
Demographics …………………………………………………………………...12
RESULTS ……………………………………………………………………………………….12
vii
DISCUSSION …………………………………………………………………………………...15
Limitations ………………………………………………………………………………17
Future Directions ………………………………………………………………………..17
REFERENCES ………………………………………………………………………………….19
APPENDICES …………………………………………………………………………………..25
Appendix A ……………………………………………………………………………..25
Appendix B ……………………………………………………………………………..28
viii
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1. Terrorism Label (aggregate)…………………………………………………………...12
ix
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1. Effect of Religion on use of Terrorism Label ………………………………………14
Figure 2. Interaction of Religion and Motive …………………………………………………15
1
Introduction
Many violent events are labeled “terrorism” by the media, but there is no definite
agreement as to what characteristics of an attack render it terrorism (Hoffman, 2013; Simmons &
Mitch, 1985). There is, although disputed, a general consensus about several characteristics
which define terrorism. Using these characteristics, terrorism can be defined as violence or threat
of violence perpetrated by an organized non-state entity, commonly a subnational group,
designed to evoke fear and intimidation in a target audience separate from the immediate victim,
in order to further political goals, where the political dimension is normally a distinctive aspect
(Hoffman, 2013; Horgan, 2014).
Some events fit the definition of terrorism, but they are not labeled terrorism (Lemieux,
2014; Nagar, 2010) For example, the perpetrator of the June 2015 shooting in Charleston, South
Carolina, in which African Americans were shot in a church, admitted that his actions were
politically-aimed and meant to spread fear in the community. Though both are distinct qualities
of terrorism, the perpetrator was not charged with terrorism (Butler, 2015). So what impacts the
label given to a violent event? When is the event a terror attack versus a hate crime? When is a
shooter mentally ill versus a terrorist? Lemieux (2014) posits that the identity of the perpetrator,
including his or her religion affects the labeling of the attack as terrorism. When the perpetrator
of an attack is a white male, he more quickly labeled mentally ill, but when the perpetrator is a
minority, he is labeled a thug or a terrorist (Butler, 2015). It may be that the perpetrator’s identity
has become more important in applying the terrorism label than the motivations or the act itself.
The media has a large influence on what the public views as terrorism. Western news
media tends to frame Islamic violence as terrorism more often, as compared to Christian or
Jewish violence (Nagar, 2010). More specifically, based on media reports, it seems that events
2
with a Muslim perpetrator are more likely to be labeled terrorism. In comparison to the
Charleston shooting, where 9 people were killed by a white male, during the July 2015 shooting
in Chattanooga, Tennessee, a Muslim perpetrator shot and killed 5 people, and the event was
labeled as domestic terrorism (Hanrahan, 2015).
Terrorism in the Media
Terrorists have been known to seek media attention for publicity, which enables them to
spread their message and intimidate their intended audience (Hoffman, 2013). Although
advances in technology mean terrorist groups no longer need to rely on traditional media outlets
to spread their message, the media still reports incidents of terrorism, frequently capitalizing on
these events to write sensationalist stories, attract more viewers, and make more money (Seib &
Janbeck, 2010). Media coverage frames the public’s perceptions of events (Nagar, 2010). As a
basic example, coverage of terrorist acts in the news gives people negative attitudes towards
terrorists, effectively giving terrorism a negative connotation (Hoffman, 2013). Exposure to
negative media about a particular group creates schemas or ingrained ideas about that group in
the mind of the public. News stories about African Americans, for instance, tend to heighten the
public’s negative perceptions and racial attitudes towards African Americans (Dixon, 2008).
Similarly, video game stereotypes can prime negative and aggressive attitudes towards Arabs
(Saleem & Anderson, 2013).
Muslims in the Media
Because of the recent surge of Islamic terrorist activity, Muslims have received very
negative news coverage. Arab Muslims are also portrayed negatively in movies, and these
images affect the public’s perception of Muslims (Shaheen, 2009). Muslims are regarded as a
3
cultural outgroup, which leads Americans to develop more negative perceptions of them
(Huesmann et al., 2012; Kalkan et al., 2009). As a result, Americans consider Muslims to be
violent, fanatical, and supportive of terrorism. The American public often associates Islam with
terrorism (Park, Felix, & Lee, 2009; Saleem & Anderson, 2013). Additionally, many Americans
have adopted a sentiment known as Islamophobia. Islamophobia is a form of cultural racism that
includes hostile attitude or behavior towards Muslims and is used justify exclusion,
discrimination, prejudice, and even violence against Muslims (Ciftci, 2012).
Female Terrorists in the Media
Gender is another variable which may impact the labeling of terrorism. Women have
been participating in terrorism for a long time (Bloom, 2011; Weinberg & Eubank, 2011). For
example, in the late 1800’s, Vera Zazulich was an active member of People’s Will, a small
terrorist group in Russia (Bloom, 2011). The infrequent appearance of female terrorists in the
media compared to that of males may affect their perception by the public. Women are not
commonly thought of as terrorists. However, they can be more dangerous than their male
counterparts. Because they do not fit the terrorist profile, female terrorists more easily evade
detection and reach their targets, yielding higher success rates than males in their attacks (Bloom,
2011). Terrorist organizations recognize this fact and exploit it, making the recognition of female
involvement in terrorism critical to counterterrorism strategy (Lavina, 2015; Nacos, 2005).
When they are shown in the media, female terrorists are portrayed without agency- as
victims of the situation instead of willing participants. For example, in explaining the
Abbottabad raid, media reports described Osama bin Laden’s wives as being innocent bystanders
when, in fact, they actively resisted the Navy SEALS who invaded the compound (Poloni-
Staudinger & Ortbals, 2014). Just as females are typically given less agency in media than males,
4
female terrorists are also shown in the media through stereotypical frames, with an emphasis on
their physical appearance and femininity, which decreases their semblance of legitimacy
compared to that of male terrorists (Nacos, 2005; Poloni-Staudinger & Ortbals, 2014). While the
media may not accurately depict women’s involvement in terrorism, the finding that Muslim
women are vulnerable to violence based on Islamophobia indicates that the public may consider
them to be connected with terrorists (Perry, 2014). There has not been a sufficient amount of
research conducted in this area, but this study will explore whether or not the public labels
females as terrorists.
Media’s Effect on American Public Policy Attitudes
The biased media coverage associating Muslims with terrorist attacks impacts the way
events are labeled as terrorism by encouraging the continuation of the same biased labeling
pattern and creating a negative feeling among Americans towards Muslims (Shaheen, 2009).
Media framing not only affects the public’s perceptions, but also their attitudes towards public
policy. Because of the prevalence of terrorism in the media, the public feels threatened and, in
turn, supports particular policies that may counter the threat (Brinson & Stohl, 2012; Davis &
Silver, 2004; Gadarian, 2010). Media images of terrorism promulgate fear, so when
policymakers present policies to counter the threat, the public is apt to support those policies. For
example, there is a lot of media coverage about the Islamic State (IS). As a result, Americans feel
threatened by IS and support military strikes in the Middle East (Gadarian, 2014).
Media coverage of terrorism influences the public’s support for policies including
military action, civil liberties, and punishment of terrorists. Exposure to negative media images
about a particular group leads to increased public support for policies against that group and
decreased support for policies that potentially help that group (Brinson & Stohl, 2012; Johnson et
5
al., 2009). Due to media representations, Muslims are widely considered by Americans to be
terrorists. Now, even without explicitly considering terrorism, an unfavorable view of Muslims
leads to support for public policies that target Muslims (Ciftci, 2012). Americans with negative
stereotypes of Muslims are more likely to support increased spending on foreign wars, defense,
and border security. They are also more likely to support decreased spending on foreign aid and
approve of the president’s handling of the situation (Sides & Gross, 2013). Media coverage with
frightening visuals about terrorism also leads to more public support for military action and
increased defense spending than neutral terrorism coverage (Gadarian, 2010).
The public’s support for civil liberty restrictions depends on how threatening they
perceive terrorists to be, as security is deemed more important than civil liberties. Attacks
labeled homegrown terrorism versus international terrorism, for example, lead to greater fear and
greater support for restricting the civil liberties of Muslims (Brinson & Stohl, 2010). Using the
terrorist label then, the government can prime the public to support a decrease in civil liberties
and gain powers such as monitoring email and phone lines and conducting home searches
(Robinson, 2008). However, the correlation between sense of threat and support for civil liberty
restrictions interacts with public trust in the government. Lower trust in government makes the
public less willing to trade off civil liberties, regardless of the threat level (Davis & Silver,
2004). Some civil liberty restrictions may be specifically directed at Muslims. For example, the
public may support the racial profiling of Muslims, restrictions on the building of mosques, and
special security screening at airports for Muslims.
Americans consider terrorism a serious problem and favor both strong measures to
prevent terrorism and strong penalties to punish offenders (Simmons & Mitch, 1985). As with
the other policies, greater fear leads to greater support for more aggressive punishment of
6
terrorists (Brinson & Stohl, 2012). Additionally, in selecting an appropriately severe punishment,
people's judgment is influenced by anchoring effects; the perceived severity of a sentence
depends on its rank with other available sentences (Aldrovandi, Wood, & Brown, 2013).
Certain characteristics about the perpetrator also affect how the public prescribes
punishment. Presumably as a result of the attraction leniency-effect, unattractive people tend to
receive more severe punishments than their attractive counterparts (Stewart, 1985). Women are
more likely to avoid charges, pretrial custody, convictions, or incarceration, and are given shorter
sentences than men (Mustard, 2001; Spohn, 2013; Starr, 2012). Nonwhites are more likely than
whites to be charged, convicted, and sentenced (Stewart, 1985). Minorities are also are more
likely than whites to be held in custody before adjudication and to receive longer sentences for
the same crimes (Mustard, 2001; Spohn, 2013). Larger than the minority-white punishment gap
is that between citizens and non-citizens. Non-citizens are charged with crimes more often and
are given harsher sentences for those crimes (Light, 2014).
A proposed explanation for this phenomenon is the minority threat perspective, claiming
that a growing number of minorities in the population threatens whites, who act to maintain
social control, and therefore alter sentencing patterns (Wang & Mears, 2010). In particular, a
larger Black population (racial threat) seems to lead to increased prison sentences and decreased
jail sentences. Similarly, a larger Hispanic population (ethnic threat) leads to more jail sentences
and fewer prison sentences or non-custodial agreements (Wang & Mears, 2010). Support for the
idea that punishment varies according to population demographics can be found even in schools.
Bekkerman and Gilpin (2016) found that schools which do not report misconducts to local law
enforcement give more severe punishments when the student body has a higher proportion of
7
minorities and students of lower socioeconomic status. Clearly then, the characteristics of an
offender will influence the public's preference of punishment.
Which Factors Affect Terrorism Labeling?
Along with religion, there are other factors which could affect the labeling of an attack as
terrorism. For example, the modality of attack, the number of victims, the gender of the
perpetrator, and the motivations behind the attack potentially impact how an attack is perceived.
The goals of the current research are to empirically test the perceptions of the American public
concerning a perpetrator’s religion, gender, and motive, how these factors affect the labeling of
attacks as terrorism, and the public’s policy attitudes as a result of the perceptions that they hold.
The first two factors (religion and gender) concern the perpetrator’s identity, while the last factor
(motive) utilizes of a defining trait of terrorism. Considering the facts that terrorism is prompted
by a political motive, Muslims have a negative media image, and female terrorists are not
credited with the same agency as male terrorists, how does the public apply the “terrorism”
label?
H1: Attacks with a male perpetrator are more likely to be labeled terrorism, compared to
the same attack by a female perpetrator.
H2: Attacks with a Muslim perpetrator are more likely to be labeled terrorism, compared
to the same attack by a Christian perpetrator.
H3: Attacks with a political motive are more likely to be labeled terrorism, compared to
the same attack without a stated motive.
H4: Use of the terrorism label is more likely to make public more supportive of policies
such as civil liberty restrictions, military action, and more severe punishment for terrorists.
8
By presenting members of the American public with an attack scenario that varies in
terms of perpetrator gender, perpetrator religion, and the presence of a motive, we examined
which factors make an event more or less likely to be labeled terrorism. These same factors, as
well as the label given to the attack, were examined in relation to the public’s resulting policy
attitudes, including support for military action, civil liberty restrictions, and punishment of
terrorists.
Method
Amazon Mechanical Turk
The participants were 1200 Americans recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk)
platform. Amazon MTurk is an online service that provides a large pool of participants available
for many different tasks at an affordable rate (Kittur, Chi, & Suh, 2008; Mason & Suri, 2012).
MTurk is therefore being increasingly used in the social sciences (Barger et al, 2015; Mason &
Suri, 2012; Paolacci & Chandler, 2014). The samples obtained through this service are more
representative of the U.S. population than samples of college students, which are frequently used
for studies (Barger et al, 2015; Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010; Behrend, Sharek, Meade,
& Wiebe, 2011; Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004; Buhrmeister, Kwang, & Gosling,
2011; Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis, 2010).
The main concern about MTurk is that participants may not pay full attention to their task
(Chandler, Mueller, & Paolacci, 2013). They may, for instance, multitask while completing a
survey. However, recent studies have shown that MTurkers actually perform better than college
students and provide data of same or slightly better quality than that of traditional subjects
(Barger & Sinar, 2011; Behrend et al., 2011; Buhrmester et al., 2011; Klein et al., 2014). High
9
quality data can be encouraged by including attention questions, which filter out those
participants who are paying attention to the survey versus those who are simply entering answers
(Oppenheimer et al., 2009; Goodman et al., 2013; Paolacci et al., 2010) Another way to promote
data accuracy is to recruit only “high reputation” workers on MTurk, whose previous work has
been evaluated as satisfactory (Berinski et al., 2012; Paolacci et al., 2010; Goodman et al., 2013).
Both the inclusion of an attention question and the recruitment of high reputation workers were
utilized in this study.
Participants opted to participate in the current study on the basis of a short description of
the task they would be completing over approximately 30 minutes. They were offered a
compensation of $1 for their participation. The invitation to the study linked participants directly
to the Qualtrics site where they completed a survey.
Survey Measures
Attack Labeling
In the first section, participants were presented with one of eight possible vignettes of a
news story depicting an attack (see Appendix A). The vignettes build on previous research which
provides empirical support for this approach and established a proof of concept (Lemieux,
Masyn, Betus Karampelas, Saleem, and Garzon, 2016; in preparation).
The news story, following a 2x2x2 design, varied key factors of the perpetrator's identity:
the perpetrator’s gender, the perpetrator’s religion, and the presence of a motive for the attack.
The perpetrator was either male or female, Christian or Muslim, and politically motivated or
without a motive. After reading the news story, participants answered a series of questions to
determine how they would spontaneously describe and label the attack and the perpetrator.
10
General Attitudes
Participants then answered questions concerning their exposure to news about Muslims,
attitudes towards Muslims, attitudes towards Christians, attitudes towards Muslim-Americans,
attitudes towards Christians in America, foreign prejudice, and female involvement in terrorism.
For these sections, each participant’s responses were compiled to form a scale score of his or her
attitude about that particular subject (see Appendix B).
Exposure to news about Muslims. Participants indicated their agreement with 6
statements (e.g. How often have you seen news stories about terrorism perpetrated by Muslims?)
on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Almost every day) to 5 (Never).
Attitudes towards Islam. Participants indicated their agreement with 6 statements (e.g.
The religion of Islam supports acts of violence.) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1
(Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Attitudes towards Christians. Participants indicated their agreement with 7 statements
(e.g. Would you feel comfortable or uncomfortable in welcoming a Christian Fundamentalist
into your home?) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly
Disagree).
Attitudes towards Muslim-Americans. Participants indicated their agreement with 10
statements (e.g. Muslim-Americans are just like any other ethnic/religious group in America.) on
a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Attitudes towards Christians in America. Participants indicated their agreement with 10
statements (e.g. Christian-Americans have America’s interest at heart.) on a 5-point Likert scale
ranging from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
11
Foreign Prejudice. Participants indicated their agreement with 10 statements (e.g. People
from other cultures are a threat to our own culture and habits.) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Female involvement in terrorism. Participants indicated their agreement with 9
statements (e.g. Women willfully engage in terrorism.) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1
(Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Policy Attitudes
In the next section, participants answered questions concerning their support for some
social and political policies, including civil liberty restrictions, military action, and punishment
(see Appendix B).
Muslims’ civil liberties. Participants indicated their agreement with 9 statements (e.g.
Suspected Muslim terrorists should have no constitutional rights, even if they are U.S. citizens.)
on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Military action. Participants indicated their agreement with 6 statements (e.g. I would
support the use of U.S. military to reduce the influence of Islam in some countries.) on a 5-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Criminal Sanctions. Participants indicated their agreement with 19 statements (e.g.
Terrorists deserve the same legal rights as everyone else.) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from
1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree).
Media Events. Participants answered 9 questions about their knowledge of recent attacks
shown in the media.
12
Demographics
In the final section, participants reported their demographic information, including
gender, age, religious preference, education level, and race/ethnicity.
Because some participants began, but did not complete the survey, 1215 participants
provided information for the initial variables (i.e. the labeling of the event and perpetrator).
However, 1200 participants provided information for all variables reported in the current study,
accounting for the difference in sample size that is reported in subsequent analyses.
Results
Participants’ responses to the spontaneous labeling questions were coded for the use of
the terms “terrorist,” “terrorism,” and relevant variants such as “terror act.” An aggregate
variable was made to account for the use of a terror label in describing either the event or the
perpetrator. This aggregate variable (terrorism label) was used for further analyses.
Table 1 Terrorism Label (aggregate)
Frequency
Percent
Yes
503
40.2
No
748
59.8
Total
1251
100.0
In order to assess the participants’ attitudes, scales were compiled from relevant items
(see Appendix B). Each scale was assessed for reliability using Cronbach’s alpha. The negative
attitudes towards Islam scale consisted of 5 items (α=.90). The negative attitudes towards
Christianity scale consisted of 7 items (α=.88). The negative attitudes towards Muslim-
13
Americans scale consisted of 9 items (α=.92). The negative attitudes towards Christian-
Americans scale consisted of 10 items (α=.72). The foreign prejudice scale consisted of 10 items
(α=.88). The attitudes towards women were divided into two subscales. The attitudes towards
women in terrorism subscale consisted of 2 items (α=.75). The attitudes towards women in the
workplace subscale consisted of 4 items (α=.60). The policy attitudes towards Muslims scale
(measured support for domestic policies that disadvantage Muslims) consisted of 9 items
(α=.91). The policy attitudes towards military action scale (measured support for military action
in predominately-Muslim countries) consisted of 6 items (α=.84). The policy attitudes towards
harsh criminal sanctions scale (measured support for harsh criminal sanctions on terrorism)
consisted of 10 items (α=.87).
The use of a terrorism label was positively correlated with negative attitudes towards
Islam (r=.11), negative attitudes towards Muslim-Americans (r=.08), support for domestic
policies disadvantaging Muslims (r=.08), support for military action policies (r=.09), and support
for harsh criminal sanctions on terrorism (r=.08). Importantly, negative attitudes towards Islam
showed a strong positive correlation with negative attitudes towards Muslim-Americans (r=.80),
support for domestic policies disadvantaging Muslims (r=.65), support for military action
policies (r=.58), and support for harsh criminal sanctions on terrorism (r=.52). Negative attitudes
towards Muslim-Americans also correlated strongly with support for domestic policies
disadvantaging Muslims (r=.71), support for military action policies (r=.63), and support for
harsh criminal sanctions on terrorism (r=.58). In contrast, negative attitudes towards Christian
Fundamentalists were negatively correlated with support for all of these policies.
Because of the link between the terrorism label and the negative attitudes towards
Muslims, an ANCOVA was used to determine under which of the conditions of perpetrator
14
identity participants were more likely to use the terrorism label, while holding these attitudes
constant. There were no main effects of either gender or motive. Holding constant the negative
attitudes towards Islam and Muslim Americans, an analysis of covariance revealed a significant
effect of religion the labeling of the event of terrorism, F(1, 1215)= 104.07, p<.001; η2 =.079.
As shown in Figure 1 below, post hoc comparisons using the Fisher LSD test revealed that
participants were more likely to label the event terrorism when presented with a Muslim
perpetrator (M= 1.51, SD= .50) as opposed to a Christian perpetrator (M=1.23, SD=.42).
Figure 1 Effect of Religion on use of Terrorism Label
The ANCOVA also revealed a significant interaction between the religion of the
perpetrator and the motive, F(1,1215)= 4.175, p<.05; η2 =.003. As shown in Figure 2 below, the
effect of religion on the terrorism label depends on the motive of the perpetrator. When the
perpetrator was Muslim, participants were more likely to label the event terrorism when the
motive was unknown (M=1.54, SE=.026) than when the motive was political (M=1.47, SE=.027).
1.51
1.23
1
1.1
1.2
1.3
1.4
1.5
1.6
1.7
1.8
1.9
2
Muslim Christian
Religion of Perpetrator
15
When the perpetrator was Christian, participants were more likely to label the event terrorism
when the motive was said to be political (M=1.25, SE=.027) than when the motive was unknown
(M=1.22, SE=.026).
Figure 2 Interaction of Religion and Motive
Discussion
Clearly, there is some bias in the way the media depicts terrorism. Attacks with Muslims
perpetrators are more likely than those with non-Muslim perpetrators to be labeled terrorism. The
effect of religion on labeling terrorism in this study suggests that media depictions have
influenced the public, generating an association between Muslims and terrorism. This effect can
be observed in the increased likelihood of participants to use the terrorism label when presented
with a Muslim perpetrator, regardless of other variables, confirming the initial hypothesis.
1
1.1
1.2
1.3
1.4
1.5
1.6
1.7
1.8
1.9
2
Muslim Christian
Religion of Perpetrator
Unknown motive Political motive
16
The correlations between the terrorism label and negative attitudes toward Islam and
Muslim-Americans imply that this “Muslim-terrorist” association may also create negative
feelings among Americans towards Muslims. These negative feelings may strengthen the
“Muslim-terrorist” association and continue to perpetuate this perception. This negative attitude
toward Muslims is extremely important because, as predicted by previous research (Brinson &
Stohl, 2012; Johnson et al., 2009), the present results also show it is associated with support for
military action in predominately-Muslim countries, domestic civil liberty restrictions for
Muslim-Americans, and harsh criminal sanctions on terrorism (see Appendix B for more specific
policies in each of these categories).
Interestingly, the gender of the perpetrator had no significant effect on the labeling. This
result does not support the initial hypothesis that a male perpetrator would be more likely to be
labeled a terrorist than a female perpetrator. This finding may suggest that, contrary to past
research findings, the public does recognize women as terrorists. More recent events, such as the
two women arrested in New York for an ISIS-related bombing plan or the involvement of the
woman with her husband in the San Bernardino attacks, may have made women’s involvement
more salient to the public.
The motive, by itself, also had no effect on the labeling of the attack. While this result
does not support the initial hypothesis, there may be a difference if participants are given a
definition of terrorism. Being in the field of terrorism studies, one knows about the inherently
political component of terrorism, whereas the general public may not. Another explanation may
be the difficulty that law enforcement may have in obtaining a motive in the aftermath of attacks.
A motive, whether reported or unknown, is therefore not seen as important for determining a
terror attack.
17
The interaction between religion and motive is interesting. The effect of religion on the
terrorism label depends on the motive of the perpetrator. When the perpetrator was Muslim,
participants were more likely to label the event terrorism when the motive was unknown than
when the motive was political. When the perpetrator was Christian, participants were more likely
to label the event terrorism when the motive was said to be political than when the motive was
unknown. Perhaps a terror motive is automatically attributed to Muslims, but for Christians, the
act may be considered terrorism only when the motive is specified as political.
Limitations
Although we acquired a large sample, because the survey was taken online, it is difficult
to ensure that the sample was representative of everyone. Some demographic information can be
obtained from the dataset. However, some people began the study and never finished. They may
have quit the survey selectively, upon seeing that it involved violence, religion, etc.
Future Directions
The survey contained many measures in addition to those analyzed in this paper. Other
variables were measured, such as the prevalence of labeling the attack a hate crime, prevalence
of labeling the perpetrator as crazy, and the knowledge participants had of the Charleston and
San Bernardino attacks. Further analyses can be used to determine whether attacks with White or
Christian perpetrators are more likely to be labeled hate crimes. Alternatively, these attacks may
be more likely to have the perpetrator labeled mentally ill.
Another study by Lemieux et al. (2016, in preparation) concerns the terrorism label.
However, it included different variables of the attack such as modality, number of victims, lone
wolf v. group, and target of attack. Comparing the results of the present study with their findings
18
may yield a bigger picture of the factors affecting the use of the terrorism label. Exploring these
aspects of the terrorism label is important because of the implications for both the depiction of
violence in the media and the formulation of public policies affecting Muslim-Americans.
19
References
Aldrovandi, S., Wood, A. M., & Brown, G. D. (2013). Sentencing, severity, and social norms: A
rank-based model of contextual influence on judgments of crimes and punishments. Acta
Psychologica, 144, 538-547.
Barger, Behrend, Sharek, and Sinar (2015). “I-O and the Crowd- Frequently asked questions
about using mechanical turk for research,” available at
http://www.siop.org/tip/oct11/03barger.aspx)
Barger, P. B. & Sinar, E. F. (2011, April). Psychological data from Amazon.com’s MTurk: Rapid
and inexpensiveBut high-quality? Poster presented at the 26th Annual Conference for
the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Chicago, IL.
Behrend, T. S., Sharek, D. J., Meade, A. W. & Wiebe, E. N. (2011). The viability of
crowdsourcing for survey research. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 114.
Bekkerman, A. & Gilpin, G. (2016). Crime and punishment: the role of student body
characteristics in schools’ disciplinary behaviours. Applied Economics, 48(15), 1402-
1415.
Berinsky, A.J., Huber, G.A., and Lenz, G.S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for
experimental research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20, 351-368.
Bloom, M. (2011). Bombshells: Women and terror. Gender Issues, 28(1/2), 1.
doi:10.1007/s12147-011-9098-z
Brinson, M. E., & Stohl, M. (2012). Media framing of terrorism: Implications for public opinion,
civil liberties, and counterterrorism policies. Journal of International & Intercultural
Communication, 5(4), 270-290. doi:10.1080/17513057.2012.713973
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source
of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 35.
20
Butler, A. (2015). Shooters of color are called ‘terrorists’ and ‘thugs.’ Why are white shooters
called ‘mentally ill’? The Washington Post. Retrieved from:
https://www.washingtonpost.com/posteverything/wp/2015/06/18/call-the-charleston-
church-shooting-what-it-is-terrorism/?tid=HP_opinion
Chandler, J., Mueller, P., and Paolacci, G. (2013). Nonnaivete among Amazon Mechanical Turk
workers: Consequences and solutions for behavioral researchers. Behavior Research
Methods, 1-19.
Ciftci, S. (2012). Islamophobia and threat perceptions: Explaining anti-Muslim sentiment in the
West. Journal of Muslim Minority Affairs, 32(3), 293-309.
doi:10.1080/13602004.2012.727291
Davis, D.W., & Silver, B.D. (2004). Civil liberties vs. security: Public opinion in the context of
the terrorist attacks on America. American Journal of Political Science, 48(1), 28_46.
doi:10.1111/j.0092-5853.2004.00054.x
Dixon, T. L. (2008). Network news and racial beliefs: Exploring the connection between national
television news exposure and stereotypical perceptions of African Americans. Journal of
Communication, 58(2), 321-337. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00387.x
Gadarian, S. (2014). How sensationalist TV storied on terrorism make Americans more hawkish.
The Washington Post. Retrieved from: http://www.washingtonpost.com/blogs/monkey-
cage/wp/2014/10/09/how-sensationalist-tv-stories-on-terrorism-make-americans-more-
hawkish/
Gadarian, S. K. (2010). The politics of threat: How terrorism news shapes foreign policy
attitudes. The Journal of Politics, (2). 469. doi:10.1017/S0022381609990910.
González, A. L., Freilich, J. D., & Chermak, S. M. (2014). How women engage homegrown
terrorism. Feminist Criminology, 9(4), 344. doi:10.1177/1557085114529809
Goodman, J.K., Cryder, C.E., and Cheema, A. (2013). Data collection in a flat world: the
strengths and weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Behavioral Decision
Making, 26, 213-224.
21
Gosling, S.D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O.P. (2004). Should we trust Web-based
studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires.
American Psychologist, 59, 93104.
Hanrahan, M. (2015). Chattanooga attack: When does a mass-shooter become a domestic
terrorist? International Business Times. Retrieved from:
http://www.ibtimes.com/chattanooga-attack-when-does-mass-shooter-become-domestic-
terrorist-2013108
Henrich, J., Heine, S.J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 33, 62135.
Hoffman, B. (2013). Inside terrorism. Columbia University Press.
Horgan, J. (2014). The psychology of terrorism. Taylor & Francis.
Huesmann, L. J., Dubow, E.F., Boxer, P., Souweidane, V., & Ginges, J. (2012). Foreign wars
and domestic prejudice: How media exposure to the Israeli-Palestinian conflict predicts
ethnic stereotyping by Jewish and Arab American adolescents. Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 22(3), 556-570.
Ipeirotis, P. (2010). Demographics of mechanical turk. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/
2451/29585.
Johnson, J., Olivo, N., Gibson, N., Reed, W., & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2009). Priming media
stereotypes reduces support for social welfare policies: the mediating role of
empathy. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(4), 463-476.
doi:10.1177/0146167208329856
Kalkan, K. O., Layman, G. C., & Uslaner, E. M. (2009). "Bands of Others"? Attitudes toward
Muslims in Contemporary American Society. Journal Of Politics, 71(3), 847-862.
Khalil, J. (2013). Know your enemy: On the futility of distinguishing between terrorists and
insurgents. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 36(5), 419-430.
doi:10.1080/1057610X.2013.775501
22
Kittur, A., Chi, E. H., & Suh, B. (2008, April). Crowdsourcing user studies with Mechanical
Turk. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing
systems (pp. 453-456). ACM.
Klein, R.A., Ratliff, K.A., Vianello, M., Adams Jr., R.B., Bahnik, S., Bernstein, M.J., and Nosek,
B.A., (2014). Investigating variation in replicability. Social Psychology, 45, 142-152.
Lavina, R. (2015). Women in terrorism: How the rise of female terrorists impacts international
law. Connecticut Journal of International Law, 30(241).
Lemieux, A. (2014). Discussion point: Why isn't anyone calling this terrorism, still? Retrieved
from: http://www.start.umd.edu/news/discussion-point-why-isnt-anyone-calling-
terrorism-still
Lemieux, A. F. & Asal, V. H. (2010). Grievance, social dominance orientation, and
authoritarianism in the choice and justification of terror versus protest. Dynamics of
Asymmetric Conflict, 3:3, 194-207, DOI: 10.1080/17467586.2010.531029
Light, M. T. (2014). The new face of legal inequality: Noncitizens and the long-term trends in
sentencing disparities across U.S. district courts, 19922009. Law and Society Review,
48(2), 447-478.
Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2011). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk.
Behavior Research Methods 43, 123.
Mustard, D. B. (2001). Racial, ethnic, and gender disparities in sentencing: Evidence from the
U.S. federal courts. Journal of Law and Economics, 44, 285-314.
Nacos, B. L. (2005). The portrayal of female terrorists in the media: Similar framing patterns in
the news coverage of women in politics and in terrorism. Studies in Conflict &
Terrorism, 28(5), 435-451. doi:10.1080/10576100500180352
Nagar, N. (2010). Who is afraid of the T-word? Labeling terror in the media coverage of political
violence before and after 9/11. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 33(6), 533-547.
doi:10.1080/10576101003752655
23
Paolacci, G. and Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a
participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 184-188.
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., and Iperiotis, P.G. (2010). Running experiments on Amazon
Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 411-419.
Perry, B. (2014). Gendered Islamophobia: Hate crime against Muslim women. Social
Identities, 20(1), 74. doi:10.1080/13504630.2013.864467
Piquero, A. R., Piquero, N. L., Gertz, M., Baker, T., Batton, J., & Barnes, J. C. (2011). Race,
punishment, and the Michael Vick experience. Social Science Quarterly, 92(2), 535-551.
Poloni-Staudinger, L., & Ortbals, C. (2014). Gendering Abbottabad: Agency and hegemonic
masculinity in an age of global terrorism. Gender Issues, 31(1), 34. doi:10.1007/s12147-
014-9117-y
Robinson, C. M. (2008). Order in chaos: Security culture as anarchist resistance to the terrorist
label. Deviant Behavior, 29(3), 225-252. doi:10.1080/01639620701498927
Saleem, M., & Anderson, C. A. (2013). Arabs as terrorists: Effects of stereotypes within violent
contexts on attitudes, perceptions, and affect. Psychology of Violence, 3(1), 84-99.
doi:10.1037/a0030038
Schanzenbach, M. M. (2004). Racial and gender disparities in prison sentences: The effect of
district-level judicial demographics. American Law and Economics Association Annual
Meetings, 1-51.
Schemer, C., Matthes, J., & Wirth, W. (2009). Media effects on group-related stereotypes and
policy opinions: Evidence from a two-wave panel survey in a political
campaign. Conference Papers -- International Communication Association, 1-43.
Shaheen, J. G. (2009). Reel bad Arabs: How Hollywood vilifies a people. Northampton, Mass.:
Olive Branch Press, 2009.
24
Sides, J., & Gross, K. (2013). Stereotypes of Muslims and support for the war on terror. Journal
of Politics, 75(3), 583-598. doi:10.1017/S0022381613000388
Simmons, C. H., & Mitch, J. R. (1985). Labeling public aggression: When is it terrorism? The
Journal of Social Psychology, 125(2), 245-251. doi:10.1080/00224545.1985.9922877
Spohn, C. (2013). The effects of the offender’s race, ethnicity, and sex on federal sentencing
outcomes in the guidelines era. Law and Contemporary Problems, 76(75), 75-104.
Starr, S. B. (2012). Estimating gender disparities in federal criminal cases. Law and Economics
Research Paper Series, 12(18).
Stewart, J. E. (1985). Appearance and punishment: The attraction-leniency effect in the
courtroom. The Journal of Social Psychology, 125(3), 373-378.
Wang, X. Mears, D. P. (2010). A multilevel test of minority threat effects on sentencing. Journal
of Quantitative Criminology, 26, 191215.
Weinberg, L., & Eubank, W. (2011). Women's involvement in terrorism. Gender Issues, 28(1/2),
22. doi:10.1007/s12147-011-9101-8
25
APPENDICES
Appendix A Survey Vignettes
Template of the vignette used in the survey:
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance
workers who were cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a man/woman later identified as
Robert Smith/Jane Smith; Fadil Azeem/ Fatimah Azeem burst through the doors of City Hall
in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Heil Hitler/Allahu Akbar.” All
five staff members were pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this
type of attack could occur in such a quiet, peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that
they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as a great shock. “My
condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time,
investigators believe the suspect had a political motive/ say the suspect's motive is unknown,
and he/she is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Christian/ Fundamentalist Islamic
community. The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in
apprehending him/her.
Conditions of the vignettes:
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a man later identified as Robert Smith burst through the doors of City Hall
in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Heil Hitler.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims, and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators
believe the suspect had a political motive, and he is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Christian community.
The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending him.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a man later identified as Robert Smith burst through the doors of City Hall
in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Heil Hitler.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. At this time, investigators say the motive is unknown. “My condolences go out to the families of the
26
victims and we are fortunate that no more people were killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released
on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators say the suspect's motive is unknown, and he is thought to be active
in the Fundamentalist Christian community. The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the
public’s help in apprehending him.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a woman later identified as Jane Smith burst through the doors of City Hall
in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Heil Hitler.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators
believe the suspect had a political motive, and she is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Christian
community. The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending her.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a woman later identified as Jane Smith burst through the doors of City Hall
in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Heil Hitler.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators say
the suspect's motive is unknown, and she is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Christian community. The
suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending her.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a man later identified as Fadil Azeem burst through the doors of City Hall in
Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Allahu Akbar.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators
believe the suspect had a political motive, and he is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Islamic community.
The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending him.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a man later identified as Fadil Azeem burst through the doors of City Hall in
Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Allahu Akbar.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
27
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators say
the suspect's motive is unknown, and he is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Islamic community. The
suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending him.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a woman later identified as Fatimah Azeem burst through the doors of City
Hall in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Allahu Akbar.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators
believe the suspect had a political motive, and she is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Islamic community.
The suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending her.
“Five people killed in local attack”
A brutal attack on a local government building early Monday afternoon killed five maintenance workers who were
cleaning the facility. Witnesses say that a woman later identified as Fatimah Azeem burst through the doors of City
Hall in Centerville, and open-fired in the lobby, right after shouting “Allahu Akbar.” All five staff members were
pronounced dead at the scene. The community was shocked that this type of attack could occur in such a quiet,
peaceful neighborhood. The mayor confirmed that they had never had a single threat and that the brazen act came as
a great shock. “My condolences go out to the families of the victims and we are fortunate that no more people were
killed in this senseless attack,” he said in a statement released on Tuesday morning. At this time, investigators say
the suspect’s motive is unknown, and she is thought to be active in the Fundamentalist Islamic community. The
suspect is currently at large and the police are asking for the public’s help in apprehending her.
28
Appendix B Attitude Scales
Table B1 Attitudes Towards Islam Scale
Items
The religion of Islam supports acts of violence.
Islam is anti-American.
Islam is an evil religion.
Muslims repress their women.
Islamic history and culture are more violent than others.
Scale reliability α=.90
Table B2 Attitudes Towards Christian Fundamentalism Scale
Items
Would you say that Christian Fundamentalists have too much or too little influence in politics?
Would you feel comfortable or uncomfortable in welcoming a Christian Fundamentalist into your
home?
Do you think that Christian Fundamentalists get too much or too little attention in the media?
How would you feel if a Christian Fundamentalist married into your family?
Do you think Christian Fundamentalists are too timid or too aggressive in publicly promoting their
ideas?
Do you support or oppose the election of more Christian Fundamentalists to public office?
Would you feel comfortable or uncomfortable if a Christian Fundamentalist became your boss?
Scale reliability α=.88
29
Table B3 Attitudes towards Muslim-Americans Scale
Items
Muslim-Americans support Islamic fundamentalism.
Muslim-Americans identify themselves as “Muslims” first and “Americans” second.
Muslim-Americans are just like any other ethnic/religious group in America.
When I think of Americans I usually don’t think of Muslims.
Muslim-Americans want to change American culture.
Muslim-Americans are likely to defend America when it is criticized.
Muslim-Americans have America’s interest at heart.
Muslim-Americans are more loyal to other Muslims around the world than they are to people in this
country.
Islamic values are a threat to the U.S.
Scale reliability α=.92
30
Table B4 Attitudes Towards Christian-Americans Scale
Items
Christian-Americans support Christian fundamentalism.
Christian-Americans have America’s interest at heart.
Christian-Americans identify themselves as “Christians” first and “Americans” second.
Christian values are a threat to the U.S.
Christian-Americans are more loyal to other Christians around the world than they are to people in this
country.
When I think of Americans I usually don’t think of Christians.
Christian-Americans want to change American culture.
I think the relationship between Christian-Americans and non-Christian Americans will remain stable
for the next few years.
Christian-Americans are likely to defend America when it is criticized.
Christian-Americans are just like any other ethnic/religious group in America.
Scale reliability α=.72
31
Table B5 Foreign Prejudice Scale
Items
We can learn a lot from people from other cultures.
It is desirable that a society should comprise people from different cultures.
People from other cultures are a threat to our own culture and habits.
It is better for a country when everybody shares the same habits and traditions.
Skin color says nothing about people.
Not all races are equally intelligent.
Foreigners (strangers) contribute to the welfare of our country.
Most foreigners are lazy and try to avoid all work that is demanding or tiring.
Foreigners (strangers) are a threat for the employment chances of Americans.
Foreigners can contribute positively to American society.
Scale reliability α=.88
Table B5 Attitudes Towards Women in Terrorism Scale
Items
Terrorism is a male activity.
Women are too soft for terrorism.
Scale reliability α=.75
Table B6 Attitudes Towards Women in the Workplace Scale
Items
Men should occupy posts of responsibility.
Some jobs are not appropriate for women.
A woman should not expect to have quite the same freedom of action as a man.
32
Women should assume their rightful place in business and all the professions along with men.
Scale reliability α=.60
Table B7 Policy Attitudes Towards Muslims Scale
Items
It is important to racially profile Muslims in order to identify any potential terrorism threat.
Suspected Muslim terrorists should have no constitutional rights, even if they are U.S. citizens.
I would support any policy that would stop the building of mosques (Muslim place of worship) in the
U.S.
In order to fight terrorism, it is acceptable for the U.S. government to listen to private conversations
without a court order.
The U.S. government should be allowed to assassinate suspected terrorists in other countries.
Police should not have to obtain search warrants when investigating suspected Muslim terrorists.
The best way to get rid of terrorism in our current “war on terror” is to weed out Islamic
fundamentalists in our country.
If we could capture or kill the terrorist group leaders we should do it, even if many civilians are injured
or killed in the process.
Muslim immigrants who visit the U.S. should have to go through special security screening procedures
at the airports.
Scale reliability α=.91
33
Table B8 Policy Attitudes Towards Military Action
Items
ISIS must be stopped by any means necessary.
The U.S. had no right to bomb Iraq.
The U.S. should not engage in any military action in Muslim countries that will kill civilians, no matter
how few.
The U.S. has to take the lead in eliminating terrorism from Muslim countries; no one else could do it.
I would support the use of U.S. military to reduce the influence of Islam in some countries.
All Muslim countries are a security threat to the United States.
Scale reliability α=.84
Table B9 Policy Attitudes Towards Harsh Criminal Sanctions Scale
Items
Punishment for involvement in terrorism should be more severe than normal criminal punishment.
Terrorists deserve the same legal rights as everyone else.
Suspected terrorists should have no constitutional rights, even if they are U.S. citizens.
Secret military tribunals for terrorists are acceptable, even if those charged are U.S. citizens.
Torturing captured terrorists for information is not acceptable.
We need to make terrorists pay for their crimes.
Terrorists who kill deserve the death penalty.
Most terrorists would kill if they are not executed or killed.
The U.S. government should be allowed to assassinate suspected terrorists in other countries.
Scale reliability α=.87