LEADING WITH MEANING: BENEFICIARY CONTACT,
PROSOCIAL IMPACT, AND THE PERFORMANCE EFFECTS OF
TRANSFORMATIONAL LEADERSHIP
ADAM M. GRANT
University of Pennsylvania
Although transformational leadership is thought to increase followers’ performance by
motivating them to transcend self-interest, rhetoric alone may not be sufficient. I
propose that transformational leadership is most effective in motivating followers
when they interact with the beneficiaries of their work, which highlights how the
vision has meaningful consequences for other people. In a quasi-experimental study,
beneficiary contact strengthened the effects of transformational leadership on call
center employees’ sales and revenue. A survey study with government employees
extended these results, supporting a moderated mediation model with perceived
prosocial impact. Relational job design can enhance the motivational effects of trans-
formational leadership.
A fundamental task for leaders is to motivate
followers to accomplish great things (Vroom & Jago,
2007). According to theories of transformational
and charismatic leadership, leaders achieve this
task by engaging in inspirational behaviors such as
articulating a compelling vision, emphasizing col-
lective identities, expressing confidence and opti-
mism, and referencing core values and ideals (Bass,
1985; Burns, 1978; House, 1977; Shamir, House, &
Arthur, 1993). Evidence suggests that when leaders
engage in these visionary behaviors, followers set
more value-congruent goals (Bono & Judge, 2003)
and experience their work as more meaningful (Pic-
colo & Colquitt, 2006; Purvanova, Bono, & Dziewe-
czynski, 2006). As a result, research has shown that
on average, transformational leadership correlates
positively with followers’ motivation and job per-
formance (Judge & Piccolo, 2004).
However, evidence suggests that transforma-
tional leadership does not always motivate higher
performance among followers. Inconsistent effects
of transformational leadership on followers’ perfor-
mance have emerged in field experiments in Cana-
dian banks (Barling, Weber, & Kelloway, 1996) and
the Israeli military (Dvir, Eden, Avolio, & Shamir,
2002), as well as in laboratory experiments using
business simulation tasks (Bono & Judge, 2003;
Kirkpatrick & Locke, 1996). One explanation for
this inconsistent evidence is that when transforma-
tional leaders articulate meaningful visions, they
face challenges in making these visions a tangible
reality. Indeed, Kirkpatrick and Locke (1996: 37)
suggested that leaders need to take steps “to ensure
that the vision is not simply rhetoric.”
In particular, a central purpose of transforma-
tional leadership is to articulate a vision that fo-
cuses employees’ attention on their contributions
to others. At its core, transformational leadership
involves “motivating followers to transcend their
own self-interests for the sake of the team, the or-
ganization or the larger polity” (Shamir et al., 1993:
579). To do so, transformational leaders often strive
to highlight the prosocial impact of the vision—
how it has meaningful consequences for other peo-
ple (Grant, 2007; Thompson & Bunderson, 2003).
However, the broad rhetoric that makes a vision
inspiring and connects it to core values may render
the prosocial impact of the vision less tangible. As
Shamir and colleagues (1993: 583) noted, transfor-
mational leadership “tends to emphasize vague and
distal goals,” yet prosocial impact is most tangible
when employees have vivid, proximal exposure to
the human beings affected by their contributions
(Grant, Campbell, Chen, Cottone, Lapedis, & Lee,
2007; Turner, Hadas-Halperin, & Raveh, 2008).
Thus, to establish the prosocial impact of a vision,
transformational leaders may need more than
words (see Kirkpatrick & Locke, 1996).
For meaningful feedback and suggestions, I thank Ja-
son Colquitt, three anonymous reviewers, Drew Carton,
Christina Fong, Dave Hofmann, Sim Sitkin, and the fac-
ulty members and doctoral students at Cornell Univer-
sity, especially Lisa Dragoni. For assistance with data
collection, I am grateful to Stan Campbell, Jenny Deveau,
Chad Friedlein, Howard Heevner, Ted Henifin, and Jon-
athan Tugman.
Editor’s note: The manuscript for this article was ac-
cepted for publication during the term of AMJ’s previous
editor-in-chief, R. Duane Ireland.
Academy of Management Journal
2012, Vol. 55, No. 2, 458–476.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0588
458
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express
written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
Although transformational leadership research
has focused on the inspirational, visionary mes-
sages that leaders deliver to followers, scholars
have recognized that leaders can also influence per-
formance by altering the structural features of fol-
lowers’ jobs (Piccolo, Greenbaum, Den Hartog, &
Folger, 2010). Accordingly, I expect that job design
is likely to play an important role in moderating the
performance effects of transformational leadership.
Rather than focusing on the traditional task charac-
teristics of jobs (Hackman & Oldham, 1976, 1980), I
focus on the social characteristics of jobs—the in-
terpersonal interactions and relationships in which
work is embedded (Grant & Parker, 2009; Morgeson
& Humphrey, 2006). Recently, scholars studying
relational job design have proposed that leaders
can enhance perceptions of prosocial impact not
only by engaging in transformational behaviors, but
also by modifying the connections between em-
ployees and the beneficiaries of their work (Grant,
2007; Grant et al., 2007). Most organizations have
prime beneficiaries—clients, customers, patients,
and other recipients or end users of their core prod-
ucts and services (Blau & Scott, 1962; Katz & Kahn,
1966). Evidence from field and laboratory studies
demonstrates that even when employees are re-
sponsible for a meaningful job or task, they gain a
stronger awareness of its prosocial impact when
they have contact with the beneficiary; this benefi-
ciary contact enables them to see the tangible,
meaningful consequences of their actions for a liv-
ing, breathing person (Grant et al., 2007). Neverthe-
less, research has yet to examine whether and how
beneficiary contact, as a key relational element of
job design, influences followers’ responses to trans-
formational leadership.
I propose that beneficiary contact strengthens the
impact of transformational leadership on follower
performance. Transformational leadership focuses
on linking a vision to core values (Shamir et al.,
1993), and research has shown that protecting and
promoting the well-being of other people is the
most important value to the majority of people in
the majority of the world’s cultures (Schwartz &
Bardi, 2001). When transformational leaders artic-
ulate an inspiring vision, providing beneficiary
contact can enhance the salience and vividness of
the vision’s prosocial impact (Grant, 2007). In this
way, beneficiary contact creates a credible link be-
tween leaders’ words and deeds (Simons, 2002),
enabling employees to see how their organization’s
mission comes to life in benefiting others, which
can motivate employees to work harder and more
effectively (Grant et al., 2007). I test these hypoth-
eses in two field studies—a quasi-experiment and a
survey study. Conducting two studies makes it pos-
sible to replicate effects across both objective mea-
sures and supervisor ratings of job performance, as
well as both temporary, experimentally induced,
and enduring, naturally occurring, differences in
transformational leadership and beneficiary
contact.
This research offers three central contributions to
theory and research on leadership and job design.
First, I introduce beneficiary contact as a novel con-
tingency for the effects of transformational leadership
on follower performance, suggesting that relational
job design can enhance—rather than substitute for—
the effects of transformational leadership on follower
performance. Second, I offer a conceptual and em-
pirical integration of research on leadership and job
design by identifying synergies between inspiring
through words (articulating a compelling vision)
and actions (designing a meaningful job). Third, I
identify a new mechanism for explaining transfor-
mational leadership effects. I show how followers’
perceptions of prosocial impact, rather than of psy-
chological empowerment, play a key role in ac-
counting for the interactive effects of transforma-
tional leadership and beneficiary contact. Together,
these advances extend classic and contemporary
discussions of how leaders’ behaviors and struc-
tural design choices operate as joint determinants
of motivation and performance (e.g., Howell, Dorf-
man, & Kerr, 1986; Yukl, 2008).
TRANSFORMATIONAL LEADERSHIP AND
BENEFICIARY CONTACT
My focus is on the effects of transformational
leadership and beneficiary contact on followers’
job performance. Performance is the effectiveness
of followers’ behaviors in advancing organizational
goals (Campbell, 1990). Transformational leader-
ship is typically conceptualized as a collection of
four dimensions of leader behavior: inspirational
motivation, idealized influence, intellectual stimu-
lation, and individualized consideration (Bass,
1985; Burns, 1978). Inspirational motivation in-
volves articulating a compelling vision of the fu-
ture. Idealized influence involves engaging in char-
ismatic actions that earn respect and cultivate
pride, such as discussing important values and be-
liefs, communicating a sense of purpose, and en-
couraging a focus on collective interests. Intellec-
tual stimulation involves challenging followers to
question their assumptions and think differently.
Individualized consideration involves personaliz-
ing interactions with followers by providing rele-
vant mentoring, coaching, and understanding. By
engaging in these transformational behaviors, lead-
2012 459Grant
ers seek to motivate employees to look beyond their
immediate self-interest to contribute to a broader
vision (e.g., Shamir, Zakay, Breinin, & Popper,
1998; Thompson & Bunderson, 2003).
To understand the factors that may strengthen
the capability of transformational leaders to accen-
tuate prosocial impact, I draw on theories of mean-
ing making and job design. Scholars have long
maintained that leaders play a critical role in man-
aging the meaning that followers make of their
work (Podolny, Khurana, & Hill-Popper, 2005; Pratt
& Ashforth, 2003; Shamir et al., 1993; Smircich &
Morgan, 1982; Thompson & Bunderson, 2003).
Transformational leadership, in particular, enables
followers to view their work as more meaningful
(Piccolo & Colquitt, 2006; Purvanova et al., 2006;
Sparks & Schenk, 2001). Inspirational motivation
highlights an important vision; idealized influence
connects this vision to important shared values;
and individualized consideration personalizes this
connection. As Shamir and colleagues (1993: 578)
explained, “Such leadership is seen as giving
meaningfulness to work by infusing work and or-
ganizations with moral purpose.”
Generally speaking, scholars have recognized
that leaders can influence followers’ perceptions of
meaningfulness through two broad sets of strate-
gies: providing messages that frame and reframe
the meaning of the followers’ work and restructur-
ing responsibilities to change and alter the mean-
ing of the work (Griffin, 1983; Molinsky & Margolis,
2005). Leadership researchers have focused primar-
ily on the former set of strategies, but job design
research has accentuated the substantial impact of
the latter. I seek to integrate the leadership and job
design literatures by examining how designing jobs
to provide beneficiary contact can amplify the ef-
fects of transformational leadership on followers’
performance.
In recent years, job design research has witnessed
a resurgence of attention to the social characteris-
tics of work (Grant & Parker, 2009; Morgeson &
Humphrey, 2006). Instead of viewing jobs merely
as collections of tasks, researchers have increas-
ingly recognized that interpersonal interactions are
critical building blocks of the work that employees
do (Oldham & Hackman, 2010; for reviews and
discussions, see Fried, Levi, and Laurence, 2008;
Grant and Parker, 2009; Kanfer, 2009; Morgeson &
Humphrey, 2008). Although a number of social
characteristics of jobs have been identified, the key
social characteristic that affects meaningfulness is
beneficiary contact—the degree to which employ-
ees have the opportunity to interact with clients,
customers, or others affected by their work (Grant,
2007). Beneficiary contact is a structural character-
istic of jobs that shapes the quality and quantity of
interactions that employees have with recipients of
their products and services (Grant & Parker, 2009;
see also Humphrey, Nahrgang, & Morgeson, 2007).
1
As Kanfer (2009: 122) summarized, “The products
of work motivation and job performance have a
relational component . . . what employees do at
work has import and meaning for others who use
the products produced or benefit in some way from
the employee’s efforts.” For example, beneficiary
contact can involve manufacturing teams interact-
ing with external customers (Kirkman & Rosen,
1999), suppliers interacting with internal custom-
ers (Parker & Axtell, 2001), radiologists having ex-
posure to patients (Turner et al., 2008), or product
developers meeting clients (Sethi & Nichol-
son, 2001).
The Moderating Role of Beneficiary Contact
Research shows that when employees have ben-
eficiary contact, they perceive greater prosocial im-
pact, as they can see and understand the tangible,
meaningful consequences of their contributions for
other people (Grant, 2007). In turn, perceived
prosocial impact is associated with higher effort,
persistence, and job performance (Grant, 2008a;
Grant et al., 2007), as focusing on meaningful con-
sequences for others can encourage employees to
continue working even when they find it unpleas-
ant (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009; Meglino & Korsgaard,
2004). However, research has yet to examine the
interplay of beneficiary contact and leadership.
I propose that beneficiary contact strengthens
the effects of transformational leadership on fol-
lowers’ performance by enhancing followers’ per-
ceptions of prosocial impact. More specifically,
1
Beneficiary contact is conceptually related to, but
distinct from, the two characteristics in the job charac-
teristics model that focus on the outcomes of work: task
identity and task significance (Hackman & Oldham,
1976, 1980). Task identity involves the extent to which
employees see their results; it does not capture the extent
to which employees interact and communicate with the
people who are affected by these results. Research indi-
cates that task identity is distinct from opportunities to
interact with customers, clients, and other recipients
(Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006). Task significance focuses
on the consequences of employees’ work for other peo-
ple; beneficiary contact focuses on the extent to which
employees have the opportunity to interact with these
people (Grant, 2007). Studies have shown that benefi-
ciary contact and task significance are empirically dis-
tinct (Grant, 2008b) and operate independently and in-
teractively to influence perceptions and behaviors (Grant
et al., 2007).
460 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
when transformational leaders engage in inspira-
tional motivation and lead by example, employees
are able to identify with an important vision (Pic-
colo & Colquitt, 2006; Shamir et al., 1993), and
beneficiary contact highlights the impact of this
vision on other people. Beneficiary contact enables
employees to see that their contributions to the
vision have meaningful consequences for other
people—that if they work harder and perform more
effectively, living, breathing human beings will be
affected positively (Grant, 2007; Grant et al., 2007).
Two complementary theoretical perspectives illu-
minate the moderating effects of beneficiary con-
tact: the availability heuristic and credibility.
According to the theoretical principles set forth
in formulations of the availability heuristic
(Schwarz, 1998; Tversky & Kahneman, 1973), peo-
ple tend to use vividness and ease of recall as cues
for probability and value. Beneficiary contact
makes the customers or clients who are affected by
a vision more cognitively accessible and emotion-
ally vivid, which will enhance employees’ beliefs
that a transformational leader’s vision is likely to
have a meaningful prosocial impact (see Heath,
Larrick, & Klayman, 1998). This understanding of
prosocial impact is likely to appeal to followers’
core values, as research has shown that benefiting
others and making a social contribution is an im-
portant value across cultures, both at work (Colby,
Sippola, & Phelps, 2001; Ruiz-Quintanilla & Eng-
land, 1996) and in life (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001).
Beneficiary contact can thereby provide employees
with a meaningful face and story to attach to a
transformational leader’s vision, creating vivid im-
agery that makes the vision more tangible (Emrich,
Bower, Feldman, & Garland, 2001).
In the absence of beneficiary contact, employees
may question the credibility of a transformational
leader’s vision, wondering whether it is merely
rhetoric. As Simons (2002: 23) suggested, “Leaders’
exhortations of a new mission or a new focus are
processed by employees as simply a new dogma or
corporate presentation, and are not translated into
action.” To overcome this gap, Kirkpatrick and
Locke (1996: 37) observed, “A leader must go be-
yond simply communicating a vision in order for it
to affect followers.” To achieve influence, it is crit-
ical for leaders to establish credibility (Lam &
Schaubroeck). Employees are most likely to per-
ceive a transformational leader’s vision as credible
when it conveys behavioral integrity—a connection
between words and deeds (Simons, 1999, 2002).
Such integrity can be established by beneficiary
contact, which has the potential to forge a vivid,
credible link between the rhetoric of prosocial im-
pact and the reality of meaningful consequences for
clients, customers, or patients. Beneficiaries can
strengthen the credibility of the leader’s vision by
providing firsthand testimonials from a relatively
neutral, knowledgeable third-party source (Grant &
Hofmann, 2011). Because they are the recipients of
an organization’s products and services, beneficia-
ries are in a unique position to articulate the proso-
cial impact of the organization’s vision (Grant &
Hofmann, 2011).
Thus, I predict that beneficiary contact enhances
the effect of transformational leadership on follow-
ers’ performance by fostering a stronger perception
of prosocial impact. In the language of social infor-
mation processing theory (Salancik & Pfeffer,
1978), transformational leadership involves pro-
viding social cues about the importance of a vision,
and beneficiary contact reinforces these cues by
allowing employees to see the potential prosocial
impact of this vision on clients, customers, or pa-
tients. Beneficiary contact aligns the design of em-
ployees’ jobs with the social cues that they are
receiving from leaders, and such alignment may
reduce uncertainty and ambiguity about the proso-
cial impact of their work (e.g., Griffin, 1983). When
transformational leaders articulate a vision, benefi-
ciary contact brings this vision to life, enabling
followers to perceive integrity in the vision and
recognize the potential for their contributions to
have a meaningful prosocial impact. The resulting
perceptions of prosocial impact, in turn, lead fol-
lowers to work harder and longer, as they perceive
effort as more worthwhile and are able to justify it
even when it is unpleasant (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009;
Grant et al., 2007; Meglino & Korsgaard, 2004).
Beneficiary contact can thus create what Weick
(1984) described as small wins, providing followers
with emotionally resonant glimpses of how small
increases in their performance can realize a leader’s
vision and have a meaningful impact on others. In
summary, I propose a moderated mediation model
in which beneficiary contact strengthens the effect
of transformational leadership on followers’ per-
ceptions of prosocial impact, which in turn contrib-
ute directly to higher performance.
Hypothesis 1. Beneficiary contact strengthens
the relationship between transformational
leadership and followers’ performance.
Hypothesis 2. Followers’ perceptions of proso-
cial impact mediate the moderating effect of
beneficiary contact on the relationship be-
tween transformational leadership and follow-
ers’ performance.
2012 461Grant
Psychological Empowerment as an Alternative
Explanation
An alternative explanation for the moderating
effects of beneficiary contact lies in theories of psy-
chological empowerment. Psychological empower-
ment is thought to involve four psychological
states: meaning (purpose), self-determination
(choice), competence (self-efficacy), and (strategic)
impact (influence on strategic, administrative, or
operating outcomes) (Spreitzer 1995; Thomas and
Velthouse, 1990). Psychological empowerment
provides a parsimonious framework for capturing
the central themes of the psychological states that
are viewed as mediators of the effects of transfor-
mational leadership on follower performance.
Transformational leadership is thought to increase
follower performance by (1) fostering meaning (Pic-
colo & Colquitt, 2006; Purvanova et al., 2006;
Shamir et al., 1993); (2) building competence or
self-efficacy (Gong, Huang, & Farh, 2009; Kirkpat-
rick & Locke, 1996; Liao & Chuang, 2007; Walum-
bwa, Avolio, & Zhu, 2008); (3) encouraging the
pursuit of self-concordant, value-congruent goals,
which are by definition self-determined and auton-
omously chosen (Bono & Judge, 2003); and (4)
strengthening social identification with a group,
department, or organization, which leads employ-
ees to “perceive themselves as important, influen-
tial, effective, and worthwhile in their organiza-
tional units” (Kark, Shamir, & Chen, 2003: 248; see
also Kirkman & Rosen, 1999).
Although these dimensions of empowerment
may mediate any direct relationship that occurs
between transformational leadership and follower
performance, I do not expect that they will be crit-
ical to explaining the moderating effects of benefi-
ciary contact on this relationship. First, meaning
can arise directly through the efforts of transforma-
tional leaders to connect work to personal values
(Bono & Judge, 2003; Piccolo & Colquitt, 2006; Pur-
vanova et al., 2006), independent of any beneficiary
contact that occurs (Rosso, Dekas, & Wrzesniewski,
2010). As Grant (2008a: 119) explains, “The expe-
rience of meaningfulness is a judgment of the gen-
eral value and purpose of the job, with no reference
to the people who it affects.” Thus, beneficiary
contact may be particularly relevant to strengthen-
ing the effect of transformational leadership on em-
ployees’ specific perceptions of prosocial impact,
whereas their more general, abstract perceptions of
meaning may be enhanced by transformational
leadership directly.
Second, with respect to competence, beneficiary
contact provides information about the impact of
followers’ work, not the extent to which they have
completed it effectively (Grant & Gino, 2010).
Third, leaders’ efforts to delegate opportunities and
provide choices influence self-determination
(Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006; Spreitzer, 1996);
since beneficiary contact is independent of auton-
omy (Grandey & Diamond, 2010), it has little rele-
vance to influencing or reinforcing the opportuni-
ties for choice that transformational leaders
provide. Fourth, although beneficiary contact pro-
vides employees with information about outcomes,
this information focuses on outcomes for the well-
being of clients, customers, and other recipients,
and is thus unlikely to affect the degree to which
transformational leadership enhances perceptions
of impact on strategic, administrative, or operating
outcomes. In summary, although psychological em-
powerment may directly mediate the relationship
between transformational leadership and follower
performance, it is less relevant as a mechanism for
explaining the moderating effect of beneficiary con-
tact on this relationship, which is likely to be
unique to perceived prosocial impact.
Overview of the Present Research
I tested these hypotheses in two studies. Study 1,
a field quasi-experiment with call center employ-
ees, examined whether establishing beneficiary
contact enhanced the effects of a transformational
leadership intervention on performance. Study 2, a
field study in a governmental organization, exam-
ined beneficiary contact as a moderator of the rela-
tionship between employee ratings of transforma-
tional leadership and supervisor ratings of their job
performance. Study 2 also compared perceived
prosocial impact and psychological empowerment
as explanatory mechanisms. In tandem, these stud-
ies facilitate the investigation of the core hypothe-
ses with respect to both temporary, experimentally
induced variations and more enduring, naturally
occurring variations in transformational leadership
and beneficiary contact.
STUDY 1: METHODS
Participants and Design
I conducted this study with new employees at a
privately held company headquartered in the U.S.
Midwest. All 71 new hires participated (response rate
100%), and 76.1 percent of them were female. The
company focused on selling educational and market-
ing software to university and nonprofit customers,
and the employees worked at an outbound call cen-
ter. The revenue that employees generated funded job
creation and salaries in another department, but they
462 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
had no contact with the beneficiaries of these jobs
and salaries. The experiment used a 2 (transforma-
tional leadership: yes, no) 2 (beneficiary contact:
yes, no) between-subjects factorial design. The em-
ployees were thus arbitrarily divided among four con-
ditions: control, transformational leadership, benefi-
ciary contact, and combined.
Procedures
To start their jobs, all employees were required to
attend a training session and were given the oppor-
tunity to sign up for one of four dates. I learned that
the manager in charge of training was planning to
invite the senior director of the organization to
speak about the company’s mission during one
training session, and he was planning to invite an
“internal customer”—a beneficiary from another
department supported by the employees’ work—to
speak about the importance of their efforts at a
different training session. Otherwise, the employ-
ees had no interaction with the senior director or
the other department. I saw this as an opportunity
for a quasi-experiment and asked the manager if he
could invite both the director and the beneficiary to
the third session. The manager agreed. Employees
in the fourth session, to which no speaker was
invited, served as the control group. Other than the
visits from the director and the beneficiary, the
training sessions were identical. Employees
were not able to self-select into conditions, as they
were not informed in advance that the training
sessions would have different speakers.
For the control group (n 26), the manager led
training without a visit from the director or the
beneficiary. For the transformational leadership
group (n 15), the director visited the training
session and spoke for 15 minutes. Exemplifying
transformational behaviors (e.g., Bono & Judge,
2003; Locke & Kirkpatrick, 1996; Shamir et al.,
1993), he articulated the company’s vision, ex-
plained why it was meaningful, and communicated
enthusiasm and confidence about employees’ capa-
bilities to achieve it. For the beneficiary contact
group (n 12), the beneficiary from a different
department visited the training session for 10 min-
utes. He described how the revenue generated by
the employees had made it possible to create jobs
and fund salaries, including his own. Finally, for
the combined group (n 18), both the director and
the beneficiary visited at different points in the
training session and delivered their messages. Both
visitors were blind to the hypotheses.
Measures
Because researcher intervention can compromise
the internal and external validity of experiments
(Argyris, 1975; Cook & Campbell, 1979; Rosenthal,
1994), I did not have contact with the participants
during the study. The manager was already track-
ing data on the employees’ performance, and I was
able to obtain these data for the seven-week period
following the intervention during training. Perfor-
mance was measured on two metrics: number of
sales made and total revenue generated. The em-
ployees were not paid on commission, but they
were eligible for semiannual salary raises based on
their performance on these two metrics. Across the
seven weekly measurement intervals, the measures
of both sales (
.82) and revenue (
.72) were
reliable. I also obtained the number of shifts
worked by each employee as a control variable.
After the seven-week performance measurement
period was complete, I gained approval to collect
manipulation check data via an online survey using
a scale anchored at 1, “disagree strongly,” and 7,
“agree strongly.” Of the 71 employees, 38 partici-
pated, for a response rate of 53.5 percent. To assess
the impact of the director’s visit, the survey fea-
tured four items from the Multifactor Leadership
Questionnaire (Avolio, Bass, & Jung, 1999). Em-
ployees evaluated the director on four transforma-
tional leadership items selected to capture the ex-
tent to which inspirational motivation and
idealized influence were reflected in his speech:
“articulates a compelling vision of the future,”
“talks enthusiastically about what needs to be ac-
complished,” “instills pride in me for being asso-
ciated with them,” and “acts in ways that builds my
respect” (
.79). To assess the impact of the
beneficiary’s visit, the survey featured two items
adapted from Grant’s (2008) beneficiary contact
scale: “My job gives me the opportunity to meet the
people who benefit from my work” and “My job
provides me with contact with the people who
benefit from my work” (
.75).
STUDY 1: RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
To evaluate the validity of the interventions, I
conducted 22 analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on
the manipulation checks. Employees who heard
the director’s speech during training rated him as
significantly more transformational (mean 5.01,
s.d. 0.95) than those who did not hear his speech
(mean 4.17, s.d. 1.38), F(1, 37) 5.25, p .05;
no other effects were significant. In addition, em-
ployees who attended the beneficiary’s visit per-
ceived greater beneficiary contact (mean 3.82,
2012 463Grant
s.d. 1.39) than those who did not (mean 2.94,
s.d. 1.22, F[1, 36] 3.99, p .05); no other
effects were significant. These results indicate sup-
port for the validity of the interventions.
Table 1 displays means and standard deviations
for the key variables by condition. To examine the
effects of the interventions on performance, I began
by conducting 22 ANOVAs on sales and revenue,
with shifts as a covariate. The results showed a
significant interaction of the transformational lead-
ership and beneficiary contact interventions on
sales (F[1, 66] 7.73, p .01). There were no
significant main effects of transformational leader-
ship (F[1, 66] .01, p .93) or beneficiary contact
(F[1, 66] .26, p .69). The results also showed a
significant interaction of the transformational lead-
ership and beneficiary contact interventions on
revenue (F[1, 66] 4.67, p .03). There were no
significant main effects of transformational leader-
ship (F[1, 66] .00, p .99 or beneficiary contact
(F[1, 66] .13, p .77).
To interpret the significant interactions, which
are graphed in Figures 1 and 2, I conducted simple
effects tests within each level of beneficiary con-
tact. When beneficiary contact was present, the
transformational leadership intervention had a sig-
nificant, positive effect on sales (F[1, 67] 4.25,
p .04 [p
one-tailed
.02]) and a marginal effect on
revenue (F[1, 67] 2.91, p .09 [p
one-tailed
.05]).
When beneficiary contact was absent, on the other
hand, the transformational leadership intervention
had no effect on sales (F[1, 67] .01, p .90) or
revenue (F[1, 67] .43, p .51). These results
provide initial evidence in support of the hypoth-
esis that beneficiary contact strengthens the effects
of transformational leadership on followers’
performance.
At the same time, these findings are subject to
several important limitations. First, the fact that the
data were collected within a single job raises ques-
tions about the generalizability of the findings to
other jobs and occupations. Second, the transfor-
mational leadership and beneficiary contact inter-
ventions consisted of short, one-time visits and
speeches. However, among working employees, a
single speech alone may be insufficient to increase
performance. In practice, transformational leader-
ship is often an everyday, repeated behavioral act
(e.g., Purvanova & Bono, 2009), and beneficiary
contact is often a relatively enduring aspect of a job
design (Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006; Stone &
Gueutal, 1985). This may help to explain why there
were no main effects of transformational leadership
or beneficiary contact.
Third, the quasi-experimental design is vulnera-
ble to validity and implementation threats (Cook &
Campbell, 1979). Although the arbitrary assign-
ment procedure prevented participants from self-
selecting into conditions, it is not possible to rule
out history threats to validity: other events may
have occurred along with the experimental treat-
ment and driven the results. Furthermore, multiple
treatment interference is a possibility: it may be the
case that simply hearing about the importance of
the work from two different sources, rather than the
specific transformational behavior by the leader
and the interaction with the beneficiary, enhanced
the credibility of the message. In terms of imple-
mentation threats, it is possible that employees
shared information about their experiences during
training, and those who did not receive both the
leader and beneficiary speeches experienced re-
sentful demoralization. Fourth, I was not able to
obtain survey data on mediating mechanisms from
participating employees. This made it difficult to
understand the underlying processes responsible
for the findings.
STUDY 2: METHODS
This study was designed to constructively repli-
cate and extend the findings of Study 1 by address-
TABLE 1
Study 1: Means and Standard Deviations by Condition
a
Condition
Number
of Sales
Total
Revenue
Sales
per Shift
Revenue
per Shift
Control (n 26) 46.23 $3,738.73 1.65 $138.61
(39.30) (3,407.49) (0.84) (75.71)
Transformational leadership
(n 15)
151.80 $12,129.04 1.55 $119.49
(102.34) (10,284.34) (0.56) (58.86)
Beneficiary contact (n 12) 77.67 $5,952.83 1.67 $131.94)
(50.68) (4,081.64) (0.74) (77.67)
Combined transformational leadership
and beneficiary contact (n 18)
271.22 $21,376.58 2.11) $166.97
(92.15) (6,806.03) (0.44) (35.73)
a
Standard deviations are in parentheses.
464 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
ing the aforementioned limitations. First, to in-
crease generalizability, I collected data from
employees in a wide range of jobs working for
different leaders. Second, to directly examine more
enduring leadership and job experiences, I col-
lected data on naturally occurring differences be-
tween employees in perceptions of transforma-
tional leadership and beneficiary contact. Third, to
overcome the vulnerability of quasi-experimental
designs to the aforementioned validity and imple-
mentation threats, I collected multisource survey
data. Fourth, to examine mediating mechanisms, I
collected data from employees on perceived proso-
cial impact and psychological empowerment.
Participants and Procedures
I collected data from 329 employees and their
direct supervisors in a large U.S. government or-
ganization. The human resources director identi-
fied 1,197 employees who had unique supervisors,
and I sent them invitations to participate in a study
of work attitudes. I received completed online sur-
veys from 418 employees, for a response rate of
34.9 percent. I sent requests to their direct supervi-
sors to complete a short online performance evalu-
ation and received completed surveys from 344
supervisors, for a response rate of 82.3 percent. I
was able to match 329 of the employee and super-
visor surveys; these matched surveys constituted
the final sample. Female employees comprised
63.5% of the sample; average job tenure was
6.3 years (s.d. 7.4), and average age was
37.2 years (s.d. 13.0). These respondents worked
in more than 20 different jobs, including engineer-
ing and manufacturing, customer service, financial
analysis, information technology, quality assur-
FIGURE 1
Study 1 Results for Sales per Shift
FIGURE 2
Study 1 Results for Revenue per Shift
2012 465Grant
ance, and legal and contracting services. Their su-
pervisors were 60.8 percent female, with an average
job tenure of 9.0 years (s.d. 8.3) and an average
age of 45.3 years (s.d. 9.7).
Measures
Unless otherwise indicated, all items used a
scale anchored at 1, “disagree strongly,” and 7,
“agree strongly.” Supervisors provided ratings of
employees’ job performance, and employees pro-
vided ratings of transformational leadership, as
well as self-reports of perceived prosocial im-
pact, psychological empowerment, and several
control variables.
Performance. To rate employees’ job perfor-
mance, the supervisors completed the five-item
scale developed by Ashford and Black (1996). They
were asked to evaluate employees’ performance on
a nine-point scale in percentiles, ranging from the
bottom 10 percent to the top 10 percent. The items
included “overall performance,” “achievement of
work goals,” and “quality of performance”
(
.96).
Transformational leadership. Employees com-
pleted the 20-item Multifactor Leadership Ques-
tionnaire (Avolio et al., 1999) with reference to
their direct supervisor (
.82). Sample items in-
clude “articulates a compelling vision of the fu-
ture” (inspirational motivation), “specifies the im-
portance of having a strong sense of purpose”
(idealized influence), “seeks differing perspectives
when solving problems” (intellectual stimulation),
and “spends time teaching and coaching” (individ-
ualized consideration).
Beneficiary contact. Since the intervention in
Study 1 consisted of a very specific interaction
with a single beneficiary, it was important to rep-
licate the results by using a measure of more en-
during and naturalistic beneficiary contact. Em-
ployees completed four items adapted from
measures developed by Morgeson and Humphrey
(2006): “My job involves a great deal of interaction
with the people who benefit from my work,” “On
the job, I frequently communicate with the people
affected by my work,” “The job requires spending a
great deal of time with the people who benefit from
my work,” and “The job involves interaction with
the people affected by my work” (
.92). These
items were based on Morgeson and Humphrey’s
(2006) measure of interaction outside a respon-
dent’s organization, but the referent was beneficia-
ries—as specified by Grant (2008b)—rather than
any people outside the organization. In pilot data,
the most commonly listed beneficiaries were cus-
tomers, citizens, and the community.
Perceived prosocial impact. Employees com-
pleted the three-item scale developed by Grant
(2008a), which includes items such as “I feel that
my work makes a positive difference in other peo-
ple’s lives” (
.81).
Psychological empowerment. Employees com-
pleted Spreitzer’s (1995) 12-item scale (
.90),
which includes 3 items each for meaning (e.g.,
“The work I do is meaningful to me”;
.81),
competence (e.g., “I have mastered the skills nec-
essary for my job”;
.89), self-determination
(e.g., “I have considerable opportunity for indepen-
dence and freedom in how I do my job”;
.86),
and (strategic) impact (e.g., “My impact on what
happens in my department is large”;
.93).
Control variables. Since the quality of the rela-
tionship between employees and supervisors af-
fects the ratings that supervisors give (e.g., Judge &
Ferris, 1993), I controlled for relationship quality to
minimize reporting biases. Employees evaluated
their relationships with their supervisors on three
items adapted from Ashford, Rothbard, Piderit, and
Dutton’s (1998) relationship quality scale, includ-
ing “trusting” and “close” (
.86). In addition, to
establish the incremental validity of the moderat-
ing role of beneficiary contact, I controlled for sev-
eral related job characteristics, using Morgeson and
Humphrey’s (2006) scales to measure task identity
(e.g., “The job is arranged so that I can do an entire
piece of work from beginning to end”;
.84), task
significance (e.g., “The results of my work are
likely to significantly affect the lives of other peo-
ple”;
.87), interpersonal feedback (e.g., “I re-
ceive feedback on my performance from other peo-
ple in my organization”;
.91), and friendship
opportunities (e.g., “I have the opportunity to de-
velop close friendships in my job”;
.89). I
selected task identity and task significance because
these two characteristics relate to the outcomes of
an individual’s job, as discussed earlier, and inter-
personal feedback and friendship opportunities be-
cause these are two other social job characteristics
that capture key aspects of employees’ work-related
interactions (Grant & Parker, 2009; Morgeson &
Humphrey, 2006).
STUDY 2: RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Means, standard deviations, and correlations for
the focal variables appear in Table 2. To assess the
factor structures of the performance, leadership, job
design, and perceptual variables, I conducted a
confirmatory factor analysis using EQS software
version 6.1 with maximum-likelihood estimation
procedures (e.g., Bentler & Dudgeon, 1996; Kline,
1998). In keeping with past research (e.g., Piccolo &
466 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
Colquitt, 2006), I constructed one parcel for each of
the four dimensions of transformational leadership.
I specified a 13-factor solution, with distinct, freely
correlated factors for supervisor performance ratings,
transformational leadership, beneficiary contact,
relationship quality, task identity, task signifi-
cance, interpersonal feedback, friendship opportu-
nities, perceived prosocial impact, and the mean-
ing, competence, self-determination, and strategic
impact dimensions of psychological empower-
ment. This 13-factor solution achieved good fit
with the data (
2
[824] 1,679.75, CFI .94,
SRMR .05). All factor loadings were statistically
significant and ranged from .73 to .97 for supervisor
performance ratings, .91 to .96 for transformational
leadership, .77 to .93 for beneficiary contact, .77 to
.86 for relationship quality, .66 to .91 for task iden-
tity, .80 to .83 for task significance, .81 to .91 for
interpersonal feedback, .76 to .95 for friendship
opportunities, .62 to .88 for perceived prosocial
impact, .88 to .97 for meaning, .80 to .94 for com-
petence, .72 to .86 for self-determination, and .88 to
.97 for strategic impact. Chi-square difference tests
showed that all alternative nested models achieved
significantly poorer fit, and constraining correla-
tions between each pair of factors to 1.0 also re-
duced model fit significantly. These analyses sup-
ported the expected factor structure of the variables.
I began testing the hypotheses using hierarchical
ordinary least squares (OLS) regression analyses,
following the moderated regression procedures rec-
ommended by Aiken and West (1991). I standard-
ized the focal variables, multiplied them to create
interaction terms, and predicted supervisor perfor-
mance ratings. I entered the control variables,
transformational leadership, and beneficiary con-
tact in step 1, the interactions of transformational
leadership with the control variables in step 2, and
the interaction of transformational leadership and
beneficiary contact in step 3. The results, which are
displayed in Table 3, indicated a statistically sig-
nificant interaction between transformational lead-
ership and beneficiary contact in predicting super-
visor performance ratings.
To interpret the form of this interaction, I plotted
the simple slopes at one standard deviation above
and below the mean of beneficiary contact (Aiken &
West, 1991). As displayed in Figure 3, transforma-
tional leadership appeared to be positively related
to supervisor performance ratings when beneficiary
contact was high but not when it was low. To test
this interpretation statistically, I compared each of
the simple slopes to zero. When beneficiary contact
was high, the relationship between transforma-
tional leadership and performance was positive
and statistically significant (b .48, s.e. .12,
.31, p .001). In contrast, when beneficiary
contact was low, the relationship between transfor-
mational leadership and performance did not differ
significantly from zero (b .10, s.e. .12,
.06,
p .40). These results show that beneficiary con-
tact strengthened the relationship between trans-
formational leadership and performance. In sup-
plementary analyses, none of the other job
characteristics interacted significantly with trans-
formational leadership, supporting the uniqueness
of the moderating role of beneficiary contact.
To examine the role of perceived prosocial im-
pact, I followed the moderated mediation proce-
dures specified by Edwards and Lambert (2007).
The hypotheses focused on first-stage moderation:
beneficiary contact strengthens the relationship be-
tween transformational leadership and perceived
prosocial impact, and perceived prosocial impact
TABLE 2
Study 2: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations
a
Variable Mean s.d. 1234567891011121314
1. Supervisor performance ratings 7.64 1.55 (.96)
2. Transformational leadership 4.54 1.28 .18 (.82)
3. Beneficiary contact 4.56 1.57 .04 .23 (.92)
4. Perceived prosocial impact 5.18 1.27 .22 .53 .30 (.81)
5. Psychological empowerment 5.54 0.91 .26 .54 .22 .62 (.90)
6. Empowerment: Meaning 5.44 1.23 .14 .44 .18 .61 .78 (.81)
7. Empowerment: Competence 6.04 0.88 .28 .20 .11 .37 .64 .34 (.89)
8. Empowerment: Self-determination 5.38 1.14 .18 .26 .21 .27 .71 .33 .39 (.82)
9. Empowerment: Strategic impact 5.23 1.46 .20 .64 .18 .56 .84 .56 .38 .45 (.95)
10. Relationship quality 5.38 1.23 .10 .51 .22 .42 .48 .43 .21 .35 .45 (.86)
11. Task identity 5.26 1.31 .09 .35 .22 .36 .48 .37 .32 .47 .40 .47 (.84)
12. Task significance 4.74 1.25 .07 .36 .36 .62 .44 .45 .22 .32 .33 .32 .32 (.87)
13. Interpersonal feedback 4.55 1.47 .11 .33 .30 .29 .36 .32 .14 .30 .28 .44 .36 .35 (.91)
14. Friendship opportunities 5.61 1.07 .16 .29 .30 .26 .36 .22 .29 .40 .28 .50 .33 .29 .38 (.89)
a
Coefficient alphas appear on the diagonal in parentheses. All r .10 are significant at p .05; all r .14, p .01; all r .18, p .001.
2012 467Grant
then contributes directly to performance. I began by
conducting moderated regression analyses predict-
ing perceived prosocial impact (see Table 4, left
column). There was a statistically significant inter-
action between transformational leadership and
beneficiary contact in predicting perceived proso-
cial impact. The simple slopes (Figure 4) suggest
that the relationship between transformational
leadership and perceived prosocial impact was
more strongly positive under high than low bene-
ficiary contact. Comparing the slopes to zero sup-
ported this interpretation: the relationship between
transformational leadership and performance was
positive and statistically significant when benefi-
ciary contact was high (b .72, s.e. .08,
.57,
p .001) and less positive but still statistically
significant when beneficiary contact was low
(b .52, s.e. .08,
.41, p .001).
TABLE 3
Study 2: Moderated Regression Analyses Predicting Supervisor Performance Ratings
a
Variables
Step 1 Step 2 Step 3
b s.e.
tbs.e.
tbs.e.
t
Transformational leadership .27 .10 .18 2.67** .28 .10 .18 2.66** .28 .10 .18 2.74**
Beneficiary contact –.07 .09 –.04 –0.71 –.07 .10 –.05 –0.74 –.02 .10 –.01 –0.20
Relationship quality –.12 .12 –.08 –1.08 –.07 .13 –.04 –0.50 –.08 .13 –.05 –0.64
Task identity .02 .10 .01 0.19 .00 .11 .00 –0.03 .04 .11 .02 0.33
Task significance –.02 .10 –.01 –.17 –.02 .10 –.01 –0.15 –.02 .10 –.01 –0.19
Interpersonal feedback .06 .10 .04 0.59 .04 .10 .03 0.39 .02 .10 .01 0.19
Friendship opportunities .23 .10 .15 2.30* .25 .11 .16 2.14* .23 .11 .15 2.01*
Transformational leadership relationship quality .16 .11 .12 1.45 .16 .11 .12 1.46
Transformational leadership task identity –.06 .10 –.04 –0.59 –.09 .10 –.06 –0.85
Transformational leadership task significance .03 .09 .02 0.33 –.02 .09 –.01 –0.17
Transformational leadership interpersonal feedback –.13 .11 –.09 –1.22 –.17 .11 –.12 –1.61
Transformational leadership friendship opportunities –.01 .10 –.01 –0.09 –.07 .10 –.05 –0.65
Transformational leadership beneficiary contact .27 .09 .19 3.07**
R
2
.05* .06 .09**
F( df) 2.51 (7, 321) 0.61 (5, 316) 9.40 (1, 315)
R
2
.02 .03**
a
Theoretically, beneficiary contact should be most likely to moderate the effects of inspirational motivation and the leading by example
aspects of idealized influence. However, the results were consistent across each facet of transformational leadership, likely because of their
high correlations (r
mean
.86, r
minimum
.83, r
maximum
.89).
b
Values shown in bold reflect hypothesized results.
* p .05
** p .01
FIGURE 3
Study 2 Simple Slopes for Supervisor Performance Ratings
468 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
TABLE 4
Study 2: Moderated Mediation Analyses
a
Variables
DV: Perceived Prosocial Impact DV: Psychological Empowerment DV: Supervisor Performance Ratings
Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2
b s.e.
tbs.e.
tbs.e.
tbs.e.
tbs.e.
tbs.e.
t
Transformational leadership .38 .06 .30 6.34*** .37 .06 .29 6.24*** .28 .05 .31 6.13*** .28 .05 .30 6.04*** .26 .10 .17 2.57* .10 .11 .07 0.95
Beneficiary contact .07 .06 .06 1.27 .10 .06 .08 1.86 –.03 .04 –.03 –0.59 –.01 .04 –.01 –0.25 –.01 .10 –.01 –0.13 –.04 .09 –.02 –0.37
Relationship quality .14 .07 .11 2.05* .15 .07 .12 2.24* .09 .05 .10 1.72 .09 .05 .10 1.82 –.10 .12 –.06 –0.83 –.16 .11 –.10 –1.40
Task identity .11 .06 .09 1.85 .13 .06 .11 2.31* .21 .04 .23 4.77*** .22 .05 .25 4.99*** .06 .10 .04 0.61 –.04 .10 –.02 –0.37
Task significance .58 .06 .46 1.06 .59 .06 .47 1.39 .17 .04 .19 3.97*** .18 .04 .20 4.05*** –.02 .10 –.02 –0.25 –.19 .11 –.12 –1.71
Interpersonal feedback .07 .06 –.06 -1.20 –.08 .06 –.06 –1.32 .04 .05 .04 0.81 .03 .05 .04 0.72 .02 .10 .01 0.22 .04 .10 .03 0.39
Friendship opportunities .06 .06 –.04 -0.90 –.06 .06 –.05 –1.00 .08 .05 .09 1.70 .08 .05 .09 1.70 .26 .10 .16 2.52* .24 .10 .15 2.31*
Transformational leadership
beneficiary contact
.16 .05 .14 3.48** .06 .04 .08 1.83 .19 .08 .13 2.39* .14 .08 .10 1.79
Psychological empowerment .30 .13 .18 2.25*
Perceived prosocial impact .20 .10 .17 2.00
R
2
.51*** .53*** .44*** .45*** .07** .11***
F(df) 47.66 (7, 320) 12.11 (1, 319) 37.07 (7, 321) 3.35 (1, 320) 3.15 (8, 319) 7.02 (1, 319)
R
2
.02** .01 .04**
a
The results did not change substantively with inclusion of the interactions between transformational leadership and the control variables. In addition, in analysis of the dimensions
of psychological empowerment separately rather than as a composite, the moderated mediation model was supported for perceived prosocial impact, but not for any of the dimensions
of psychological empowerment. This is not surprising in light of evidence that the dimensions tend to be highly correlated and share similar antecedents and outcomes (Seibert, Wang,
& Courtright, 2011).
b
Values shown in bold reflect hypothesized results.
* p .05,
** p .01
*** p .001
Next, I tested whether perceived prosocial im-
pact predicted supervisor performance ratings
when transformational leadership, beneficiary con-
tact, and their interaction were controlled. I con-
ducted these analyses while controlling for psycho-
logical empowerment (Table 4, right column). In
both analyses, perceived prosocial impact was a
significant predictor even after I had controlled for
these variables, and the coefficient on the interac-
tion term decreased below statistical significance.
To examine whether this was a significant de-
crease, I used bootstrap procedures to construct
95% bias-corrected confidence intervals around
the indirect effects at both levels of beneficiary
contact (Edwards & Lambert, 2007). The confidence
interval for the indirect effect of transformational
leadership on supervisor performance ratings
through perceived prosocial impact excluded zero
for both high beneficiary contact (.05, .29) and low
beneficiary contact (.01, .15), indicating that per-
ceived prosocial impact mediated the relationship
between transformational leadership and follower
performance at both levels of beneficiary contact.
2
In addition, the confidence interval for the differ-
ence between these two indirect effects excluded
zero (.02, .22), indicating that the indirect effect
was significantly stronger under high rather than
low beneficiary contact. These results support the
moderated mediation model, showing that per-
ceived prosocial impact is an explanatory mecha-
nism even after one controls for psychological
empowerment.
Psychological empowerment independently pre-
dicted supervisor performance ratings (Table 4,
right column), but beneficiary contact did not mod-
erate the relationship of transformational leader-
ship with psychological empowerment (Table 4,
middle column). These findings support the pre-
diction that perceived prosocial impact, rather than
psychological empowerment, is a key mechanism
through which beneficiary contact strengthens the
relationship between transformational leadership
and follower performance.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
These studies provide convergent evidence that
the relationship between transformational leader-
ship and follower performance is stronger under
beneficiary contact. In the first study, a transforma-
tional leadership intervention enhanced sales and
revenue, but only when employees had contact
with a beneficiary. In the second study, the positive
association between transformational leadership
and supervisor ratings of follower performance was
stronger under beneficiary contact, and followers’
perceptions of prosocial impact mediated this in-
teractive relationship.
Theoretical Contributions
This research advances knowledge about leader-
ship, job design, and meaning. The primary contri-
bution lies in introducing beneficiary contact as an
important moderator of the impact of transforma-
2
Under high beneficiary contact, the confidence inter
-
vals excluded zero for both the direct effects (.03, .58)
and the total effects (.20, .73). Under low beneficiary
contact, on the other hand, the confidence intervals in-
cluded zero for both the direct effects (–.26, .37) and the
total effects (–.21, .42).
FIGURE 4
Study 2 Simple Slopes for Perceived Prosocial Impact
470 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
tional leadership on follower performance. Al-
though evidence has accumulated that both trans-
formational leadership and beneficiary contact can
motivate employees to perform more effectively,
little theory and research have examined the inter-
play between these two approaches to imbuing
work with meaning. In identifying beneficiary con-
tact as an enhancer of the effects of transforma-
tional leadership, my theoretical perspective and
empirical findings represent a departure from tra-
ditional approaches to understanding the interac-
tions of leadership and job design. In classic re-
search, the assumption has been that job design is a
substitute for leadership: well-designed tasks com-
pensate for the absence of leadership behaviors by
providing employees with the intrinsic motivation
and direction necessary to complete their work ef-
fectively regardless of vision and inspiration from
leaders (Kerr & Jermier, 1978). Some studies have
supported this perspective (Dionne, Yammarino,
Atwater, & James, 2002; Keller, 2006; cf. Podsakoff,
MacKenzie, & Bommer, 1996), but this research has
focused on task characteristics, giving little theoret-
ical and empirical attention to the social character-
istics of jobs. From a leadership substitutes per-
spective, one might expect beneficiary contact to
serve a compensatory function, fostering percep-
tions of prosocial impact when transformational
leadership is lacking. However, my research sup-
ports the opposite functional form of the interac-
tion. These studies thereby open up a new direc-
tion for leadership research, suggesting that
although task characteristics may be substitutes for
leadership, social characteristics of jobs may be
more likely to operate as enhancers.
The studies also highlight the potential for re-
thinking and broadening existing knowledge about
the behaviors of transformational leaders. As it is
traditionally studied, the inspirational motivation
dimension of transformational leadership focuses
on the use of language and rhetoric to instill enthu-
siasm, optimism, confidence, and purpose in fol-
lowers (e.g., Avolio et al., 1999; Bass, 1985; Emrich
et al., 2001; Shamir et al., 1993, 1998). My research
suggests that transformational leadership may also
involve modifying the structural designs of follow-
ers’ jobs. This evidence points to a novel interpre-
tation of recent studies linking transformational
leadership to perceptions of job enrichment and
meaningfulness (Piccolo & Colquitt, 2006; Pur-
vanova et al., 2006). Whereas these researchers
have assumed that transformational leaders influ-
ence employees’ job perceptions and performance
through the rhetoric that they use, my research
indicates that transformational leaders can also
achieve such influence through objectively altering
the design of employees’ jobs to create greater in-
teraction with beneficiaries. My studies suggest
that rhetoric and design in combination, rather
than one or the other alone, may maximize the
extent to which followers perceive their work as
having a prosocial impact and perform effectively
as a result. As such, my research takes a step toward
answering recent calls to better understand the inter-
play of job design and leadership (Piccolo et al., 2010)
and draws attention to relational job design as a
moderator of transformational leadership effects,
complementing previously studied contingencies
such as environmental uncertainty, cultural values,
social and physical distance, and follower charac-
teristics (for reviews, see Bass & Riggio, 2006; Avo-
lio, Walumbwa, & Weber, 2009).
Accordingly, my findings invite scholars to con-
sider the possibility that transformational leaders
can inspire employees not only through the words
that they articulate to link work to an important
purpose, but also through the actions that they
undertake to redesign this work to strengthen con-
nections to this purpose. Recent research shows
that it is difficult for leaders to create perceptions of
prosocial impact through their own words; mes-
sages highlighting prosocial impact are more com-
pelling when delivered directly by beneficiaries
(Grant & Hofmann, 2011). Should connecting em-
ployees with beneficiaries outside their work
groups be viewed as a transformational leadership
behavior? If so, it may be fruitful to conceive of
transformational leadership as a form of boundary
management in which leaders close the gap be-
tween employees and beneficiaries, serving as link-
ing pins (Katz & Kahn, 1966) to bridge “structural
holes” between employees and beneficiaries (Burt,
1997; Obstfeld, 2005).
My research also identifies perceived prosocial
impact as a new mechanism for explaining trans-
formational leadership effects. As discussed previ-
ously, existing research has focused on how trans-
formational leadership operates through meaning,
self-concordance, competence or self-efficacy, and
social identification. These mechanisms focus on
employees’ perceptions of their work, their own
capabilities, and their relationships with leaders
and work group members. Perceived prosocial im-
pact differs from these mechanisms in that it pri-
marily emphasizes employees’ perceptions of their
relationships with beneficiaries outside their work
groups. My research thus introduces a fresh under-
standing of how transformational leadership can
shape performance by influencing how employees
judge their relationships with the recipients of their
products and services, not only their relationships
with leaders and employees inside their work
2012 471Grant
groups. This theoretical perspective and empirical
evidence widen the relational scope of transforma-
tional leadership effects.
Finally, my research extends current knowledge
about the psychological and performance effects of
relational job design, which scholars have identi-
fied as a productive direction for future research
(Grant & Parker, 2009; Kanfer, 2009; Morgeson &
Humphrey, 2008; Oldham & Hackman, 2010). Pre-
vious studies have shown how beneficiary contact,
independently and in conjunction with supporting
task characteristics, can enhance attitudes and per-
formance (Grant, 2007; Humphrey et al., 2007). Lit-
tle research, however, has addressed how factors
other than job design interact with beneficiary con-
tact to affect employees’ psychological and behav-
ioral reactions. My studies provide what may be the
first evidence that beneficiary contact interacts
with leadership to influence perceptions of proso-
cial impact and performance. These findings sug-
gest that to develop a comprehensive understand-
ing of the impact of relational job design on
employees, it is important to examine leadership
behaviors in tandem with job characteristics.
Limitations, Future Directions, and Practical
Implications
These studies are subject to a number of limita-
tions that point toward avenues for future research.
One inconsistency between the two studies con-
cerned the effect of transformational leadership on
performance under low beneficiary contact; this
effect was insignificant in both studies but showed
a negative trend (Study 1) versus a positive trend
(Study 2). Future research is necessary to compare
a number of possible explanations for this diver-
gence, including differences in the focus on em-
ployees in for-profit versus governmental organiza-
tions, objective versus supervisor ratings of
performance, and temporary versus enduring lead-
ership behaviors and job characteristics. It may be
the case that small doses of transformational lead-
ership depend heavily on beneficiary contact to
make the consequences of a leader’s vision for
other people tangible, whereas when transforma-
tional leadership is a salient component of every-
day work life, beneficiary contact has incremental
value but is not strictly necessary. Alternatively,
the employees in the Study 2 sample who reported
low beneficiary contact may still have had suffi-
cient interaction with customers and clients that
they were able to vividly understand and envision
the impact of their organization’s work. As an
anonymous reviewer for this article noted, these
two studies do not rule out the possibility that job
design and organizational culture can be a substi-
tute for transformational leadership. For some jobs,
organizations, and occupations, the work may be so
deeply imbued with ideological significance that
its prosocial impact is vivid and chronically salient
to employees. In these situations, it may not be
necessary or beneficial for leaders to provide addi-
tional inspiration or to redesign jobs.
On a related note, in both studies, beneficiary
contact was not independently associated with
higher performance. This raises important ques-
tions about whether the effects of beneficiary con-
tact vary as a function of its structure and content
(Grandey & Diamond, 2010; Grant & Parker, 2009).
For example, a beneficiary’s need, similarity, emo-
tional expressions, responsibility, charisma, au-
thenticity, and attractiveness may be important
contingencies that affect employees’ reactions (e.g.,
Batson & Shaw, 1991; Grant et al., 2007; Small &
Verrochi, 2009), and I did not measure or manipu-
late these potential contingencies in the present
studies. The match or fit between a beneficiary and
a leader’s vision is also likely to be important. More
generally, beneficiary contact is only one of multi-
ple social characteristics of work, and it will be
valuable to gain a deeper understanding of the po-
tential moderating effects of other social character-
istics, such as task interdependence, social support
and undermining, requirements for harm doing,
and accountability (Grant & Parker, 2009; Hum-
phrey et al., 2007).
In Study 1, the results may have been partially
influenced by the fact that two different speakers
reinforced the message. Although Study 2 offset
this limitation by using employees’ ratings of on-
going levels of transformational leadership and
beneficiary contact, future experimental studies
should independently vary the number of messages
and their source. Another limitation is that the
studies provide little insight into the duration of
the interactive performance effects of transforma-
tional leadership and beneficiary contact. The first
study was limited to seven weeks of performance
measurement, and the second study included only
cross-sectional data. Since the effects of motiva-
tional interventions often fade over time (e.g., Mc-
Natt & Judge, 2004), it will be critical to build, test,
and refine theory about how beneficiary contact
influences the sustainability of performance
changes over time, as well as to test the underlying
availability and credibility mechanisms implied in
the theory development.
I was unable to track differences among the di-
mensions of transformational leadership (cf.
Shamir et al., 1998) and in the types of social cues
provided (Zalesny & Ford, 1990). These shortcom-
472 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
ings raise unanswered questions about the specific
behaviors of transformational leaders that are en-
hanced by beneficiary contact. In addition, future
research should manipulate and measure other
leadership constructs—such as leader-member ex-
change, empowering leadership, and authentic
leadership (for a review, see Avolio et al.
[2009])—to address the extent to which the moder-
ating role of beneficiary contact is unique to trans-
formational leadership. Evaluating Study 2 in iso-
lation, it is difficult to ascertain whether the effects
are driven by the leader’s behavior, the follower’s
perception of the leader, or a combination of the
two. This limitation is partially offset by Study 1,
which shows that objective leadership behaviors
interact with beneficiary contact to influence per-
formance, but future research should include mul-
tiple followers per leader to demonstrate consensus
in follower ratings. This may yield greater discrim-
inant validity between facets of transformational
leadership and inform whether the moderating ef-
fects of beneficiary contact apply primarily to in-
spirational motivation and idealized influence.
These limitations notwithstanding, the present
research shows how beneficiary contact can en-
hance the performance effects of transformational
leadership. Transformational leaders may bring
prosocial visions to life by establishing contact be-
tween employees and beneficiaries (see Grant,
2011). As Medtronic’s former CEO Bill George (in a
2010 personal communication) reflected:
Medtronic’s mission is not fulfilled until the person
is restored to full life and health, even with chronic
and intractable diseases. They need to remember
that when they get frustrated, they’re here to restore
people to full life and health. If I’m making semi-
conductors, how do I get to see the impact on pa-
tients? If I’m doing software development, if there
was a glitch in a defibrillator, people could be
harmed or killed. . . . Medtronic covers two out of
every three surgeries with someone in the room—
salespeople, technicians, clinical specialists. . . . It’s
very important that they get out there and see pro-
cedures. . . . You get to see the patients firsthand . . .
it’s a way of communicating what we’re all about.
REFERENCES
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. 1991. Multiple regression:
Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Argyris, C. 1975. Dangers in applying results from exper-
imental social psychology. American Psychologist,
30: 469485.
Ashford, S. J., & Black, J. S. 1996. Proactivity during
organizational entry: Antecedents, tactics, and out-
comes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81: 199
214.
Ashford, S. J., Rothbard, N. P., Piderit, S. K., & Dutton,
J. E. 1998. Out on a limb: The role of context and
impression management in spelling gender equity
issues. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43: 23–
57.
Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M., & Jung, D. I. 1999. Re-examining
the components of transformational and transac-
tional leadership using the Multifactor Leadership
Questionnaire. Journal of Occupational and Or-
ganizational Psychology, 72: 441–462.
Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Weber, T. J. 2009.
Leadership: Current theories, research, and future
directions. In Annual review of psychology, vol. 60:
421–449. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Barling, J., Weber, T., & Kelloway, E. K. 1996. Effects of
transformational leadership training on attitudinal
and financial outcomes: A field experiment. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 81: 827– 832.
Bass, B. M. 1985. Leadership and performance beyond
expectations. New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. 2006. Transformational lead-
ership (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. 1991. Evidence for altruism:
Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives. Psycholog-
ical Inquiry, 2: 107–122.
Bentler, P. M., & Dudgeon, P. 1996. Covariance structure
analysis: Statistical practice, theory, and directions.
In J. T. Spence, J. M. Darley, & D. J. Foss (Eds.),
Annual review of psychology, vol. 47: 563–592.
Blau, P. M., & Scott, W. R. 1962. Formal organizations.
San Francisco: Chandler.
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. 2003. Self-concordance at
work: Toward understanding the motivational ef-
fects of transformational leaders. Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 46: 554–571.
Burns, J. M. 1978. Leadership. New York: Harper & Row.
Burt, R. S. 1997. The contingent value of social capital.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 42: 339 –365.
Campbell, J. P. 1990. Modeling the performance predic-
tion problem in industrial and organizational psy-
chology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.),
Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-
chology (2nd ed.): 687–732. Palo Alto, CA: Consult-
ing Psychologists Press.
Colby, A., Sippola, L., & Phelps, E. 2001. Social respon-
sibility and paid work in contemporary American
life. In A. Rossi (Ed.), Caring and doing for others:
Social responsibility in the domains of family,
work, and community: 463–501. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
2012 473Grant
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. 1979. Quasi-experimen-
tation: Design and analysis issues for field set-
tings. Chicago: Rand McNally.
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nauta, A. 2009. Self-interest and
other-orientation in organizational behavior: Impli-
cations for job performance, prosocial behavior, and
personal initiative. Journal of Applied Psychology,
94: 913–926.
Dionne, S. D., Yammarino, F. J., Atwater, L. E., & James,
L. R. 2002. Neutralizing substitutes for leadership
theory: Leadership effects and common-source bias.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 87: 454 464.
Dvir, T., Eden, D., Avolio, B. J., & Shamir, B. 2002. Impact
of transformational leadership on follower develop-
ment and performance: A field experiment. Acad-
emy of Management Journal, 45: 735–744.
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. 2007. Methods for inte-
grating moderation and mediation: A general analyt-
ical framework using moderated path analysis. Psy-
chological Methods, 12: 1–22.
Emrich, C. G., Brower, H. H., Feldman, J. M., & Garland,
H. 2001. Images in words: Presidential rhetoric, cha-
risma, and greatness. Administrative Science Quar-
terly, 56: 527–557.
Fried, Y., Levi, A. S., & Laurence, G. 2008. Motivation
and job design in the new world of work. In S.
Cartwright & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The Oxford hand-
book of personnel psychology, 24: 586611. Ox-
ford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
Gong, Y., Huang, J.-C., & Farh, J.-L. 2009. Employee learn-
ing orientation, transformational leadership, and
employee creativity: The mediating role of employee
creative self-efficacy. Academy of Management
Journal, 52: 765–778.
Grandey, A. A., & Diamond, J. A. 2010. Interactions with
the public: Bridging job design and emotional labor
perspectives. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
31: 338–350.
Grant, A. M. 2007. Relational job design and the motiva-
tion to make a prosocial difference. Academy of
Management Review, 32: 393–417.
Grant, A. M. 2008a. The significance of task significance:
Job performance effects, relational mechanisms, and
boundary conditions. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 93: 108–124.
Grant, A. M. 2008b. Designing jobs to do good: Dimen-
sions and psychological consequences of prosocial
job characteristics. Journal of Positive Psychology,
3: 19–39.
Grant, A. M. 2011. How customers can rally your troops:
End users can energize your workforce far better
than your managers can. Harvard Business Review,
89(6): 97–103.
Grant, A. M., Campbell, E. M., Chen, G., Cottone, K.,
Lapedis, D., & Lee, K. 2007. Impact and the art of
motivation maintenance: The effects of contact with
beneficiaries on persistence behavior. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
103: 53–67.
Grant, A. M., & Gino, F. 2010. A little thanks goes a long
way: Explaining why gratitude expressions motivate
prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 98: 946–955.
Grant, A. M., & Hofmann, D. A. 2011. Outsourcing inspi-
ration: The performance effects of ideological mes-
sages from leaders and beneficiaries. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
116: 173–187.
Grant, A. M., & Parker, S. K. 2009. Redesigning work
design theories: The rise of relational and proactive
perspectives. In J. P. Walsh & A. P. Brief (Eds.),
Academy of Management annals, vol. 3: 317–375.
Essex, U.K.: Routledge.
Griffin, R. W. 1983. Objective and social sources of in-
formation in task redesign: A field experiment. Ad-
ministrative Science Quarterly, 28: 184 –200.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1976. Motivation
through the design of work: Test of a theory. Or-
ganizational Behavior and Human Performance,
16: 250–279.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1980. Work redesign.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Klayman, J. 1998. Cognitive
repairs: How organizations compensate for the short-
coming of individual learners. In B. Staw & R. Sutton
(Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol.
20: 1–37. Greenwich, CT: JAI.
House, R. J. 1977. A 1976 theory of charismatic leader-
ship. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larsen, (Eds.), Leadership:
The cutting edge: 189–207. Carbondale: Southern
Illinois University Press.
Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., & Kerr, S. 1986. Moderator
variables in leadership research. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 11: 88–102.
Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. 2007.
Integrating motivational, social, and contextual work
design features: A meta-analytic summary and theo-
retical extension of the work design literature. Jour-
nal of Applied Psychology, 92: 1332–1356.
Judge, T. A., & Ferris, G. R. 1993. Social context of per-
formance evaluation decisions. Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 36: 80–105.
Judge, T. A., & Piccolo, R. F. 2004. Transformational and
transactional leadership: A meta-analytic test of
their relative validity. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 89: 755–768.
Kanfer, R. 2009. Work motivation: Advancing theory and
474 AprilAcademy of Management Journal
impact. Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
2: 118–127.
Kark, R., Shamir, B., & Chen, G. 2003. The two faces of
transformational leadership: Empowerment and de-
pendency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88: 246
255.
Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. 1966. The social psychology of
organizations. New York: Wiley.
Keller, R. T. 2006. Transformational leadership, initiat-
ing structure, and substitutes for leadership: A lon-
gitudinal study of research and development project
team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
91: 202–210.
Kerr, S., & Jermier, J. 1978. Substitutes for leadership:
Their meaning and measurement. Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance, 22: 375– 403.
Kirkman, B. L., & Rosen, B. 1999. Beyond self-manage-
ment: Antecedents and consequences of team em-
powerment. Academy of Management Journal, 42:
58–74.
Kirkpatrick, S. A., & Locke, E. A. 1996. Direct and indi-
rect effects of three core charismatic leadership com-
ponents on performance and attitudes. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 81: 36–51
Kline, R. B. 1998. Principles and practice of structural
equation modeling. New York: Guilford.
Lam, S. S. K., & Schaubroeck, J. 2000. A field experiment
testing frontline opinion leaders as change agents.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 85: 987–995.
Liao, H., & Chuang, A. 2007. Transforming service em-
ployees and climate: A multilevel, multisource ex-
amination of transformational leadership in building
long-term service relationships. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 92: 1006–1019.
McNatt, D. B., & Judge, T. A. 2004. Boundary conditions
of the Galatea effect: A field experiment and con-
structive replication. Academy of Management
Journal, 47: 550–565.
Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. 2004. Considering
rational self-interest as a disposition: Organizational
implications of other orientation. Journal of Ap-
plied Psychology, 89: 946–959.
Molinsky, A., & Margolis, J. 2005. Necessary evils and
interpersonal sensitivity in organizations. Academy
of Management Review, 30: 245–268.
Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. 2006. The Work
Design Questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and vali-
dating a comprehensive measure for assessing job
design and the nature of work. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 91: 1321–1339.
Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. 2008. Job and team
design: Toward a more integrative conceptualization
of work design. In J. Martocchio (Ed.), Research in
personnel and human resources management, vol.
27: 39–92. Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Obstfeld, D. 2005. Social networks, the tertius iungens
orientation, and involvement in innovation. Admin-
istrative Science Quarterly, 50: 100 –130.
Oldham, G. R., & Hackman, J. R. 2010. Not what it was
and not what it will be: The future of job design
research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31:
463–479.
Parker, S. K., & Axtell, C. M. 2001. Seeing another view-
point: Antecedents and outcomes of employee per-
spective taking. Academy of Management Journal,
44: 1085–1100.
Piccolo, R. F., & Colquitt, J. A. 2006. Transformational
leadership and job behaviors: The mediating role of
core job characteristics. Academy of Management
Journal, 49: 327–340.
Piccolo, R., Greenbaum, R., Den Hartog, D., & Folger, R.
2010. The relationship between ethical leadership
and core job characteristics. Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 31: 259–278.
Podolny, J. M., Khurana, R., & Hill-Popper, M. 2005.
Revisiting the meaning of leadership. In B. Staw &
R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organizational
behavior, vol. 26: 1–36. New York: Elsevier Science.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Bommer, W. H.
1996. Meta-analysis of the relationships between
Kerr and Jermier’s substitutes for leadership and em-
ployee job attitudes, role perceptions, and perfor-
mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81: 380
399.
Pratt, M. G., & Ashforth, B. E. 2003. Fostering meaning-
fulness in working and at work. In K. Cameron, J. E.
Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organiza-
tional scholarship: Foundations of a new disci-
pline: 308–327. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
Purvanova, R. K., & Bono, J. E. 2009. Transformational
leadership in context: Face-to-face and virtual teams.
Leadership Quarterly, 20: 343–357.
Purvanova, R. K., Bono, J. E., & Dzieweczynski, J. 2006.
Transformational leadership, job characteristics, and
organizational citizenship performance. Human
Performance, 19: 1–22.
Rosenthal, R. 1994. Interpersonal expectancy effects: A
30-year perspective. Current Directions in Psycho-
logical Science, 3: 176–179.
Rosso, B., Dekas, K., & Wrzesniewski, A. 2010. On the
meaning of work: A theoretical integration and re-
view. In Research in organizational behavior, vol.
30: 91–127. .
Ruiz-Quintanilla, S. A., & England, G. W. 1996. How
working is defined: Structure and stability. Journal
of Organizational Behavior, 17: 515–540.
2012 475Grant
Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. 1978. A social information
processing approach to job attitudes and task design.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 23: 224 –253.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. 2001. Value hierarchies
across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32: 268 –290.
Schwarz, N. 1998. Accessible content and accessibility
experiences: The interplay of declarative and expe-
riential information in judgment. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 2: 87–99.
Seibert, S. E., Wang, G., & Courtright, S. H. 2011. Ante-
cedents and consequences of psychological and
team empowerment in organizations: A meta-ana-
lytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96:
981–1003.
Sethi, R., & Nicholson, C. Y. 2001. Structural and con-
textual correlates of charged behavior in product
development teams. Journal of Product Innovation
Management, 18(3): 154–168.
Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. 1993. The mo-
tivational effects of charismatic leadership: A self-
concept based theory. Organization Science, 4:
577–594.
Shamir, B., Zakay, E., Breinin, E., & Popper, M. 1998.
Correlates of charismatic leader behavior in military
units: Subordinates’ attitudes, unit characteristics,
and superiors’ appraisals of leader performance.
Academy of Management Journal, 41: 387– 409.
Simons, T. L. 1999. Behavioral integrity as a critical
ingredient for transformational leadership. Journal
of Organizational Change Management, 12: 89
104.
Simons, T. 2002. Behavioral integrity: The perceived
alignment between managers’ words and deeds as a
research focus. Organization Science, 13: 18 –35.
Small, D. A., & Verrochi, N. M. 2009. The face of need:
Facial emotion expression on charity advertise-
ments. Journal of Marketing Research, 46: 777–
787.
Smircich, L., & Morgan, G. 1982. Leadership: The man-
agement of meaning. Journal of Applied Behavioral
Science, 18: 257–273.
Sparks, J. R., & Schenk, J. A. 2001. Explaining the effects
of transformational leadership: An investigation of
the effects of higher-order motives in multilevel mar-
keting organizations. Journal of Organizational Be-
havior, 22: 849869.
Spreitzer, G. M. 1995. Psychological empowerment in
the workplace: Dimensions, measurement, and vali-
dation. Academy of Management Journal, 38:
1442–1465.
Spreitzer, G. M. 1996. Social structural characteristics of
psychological empowerment. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 39: 483–504.
Thomas, K. W., & Velthouse, B. A. 1990. Cognitive ele-
ments of empowerment: An “interpretive” model of
intrinsic task motivation. Academy of Management
Review, 15: 666681.
Thompson, J. A., & Bunderson, J. S. 2003. Violations of
principle: Ideological currency in the psychological
contract. Academy of Management Review, 28:
571–586.
Turner, Y. N., Hadas-Halperin, I., & Raveh, D. 2008. Pa-
tient photos spur radiologist empathy and eye for
detail. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Radiological Society of North America, Chicago.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. 1973. Availability: A heu-
ristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogni-
tive Psychology, 5: 207–232.
Vroom, V. H., & Jago, A. G. 2007. The role of the situation
in leadership. American Psychologist, 62: 17–24.
Walumbwa, F. O., Avolio, B. J., & Zhu, W. 2008. How
transformational leadership weaves its influence on
individual job performance: The role of identifica-
tion and efficacy beliefs. Personnel Psychology, 61:
793–825.
Weick, K. E. 1984. Small wins: Redefining the scale of
social problems. American Psychologist, 39: 40 49.
Yukl, G. 2008. How leaders influence organizational ef-
fectiveness. Leadership Quarterly, 19: 708 –722.
Zalesny, M. D., & Ford, J. K. 1990. Extending the social
information processing perspective: New links to at-
titudes, behaviors, and perceptions. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47: 205–
246.
Adam M. Grant ([email protected]) is an as-
sociate professor of management at the Wharton School
at the University of Pennsylvania. He received his Ph.D.
in organizational psychology from the University of
Michigan. His research focuses on work motivation, job
design, prosocial helping and giving behaviors, and pro-
active behaviors.
476 AprilAcademy of Management Journal